Attenuation of perceived motion smear during the vestibulo-ocular reflex

Previous studies indicated that less motion smear is perceived when a physically stationary target is presented during voluntary eye movements than when similar retinal-image motion occurs during steady fixation. In this study, we assessed whether the perception of motion smear is attenuated also during the involuntary vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR). Normal observers matched the length of perceived smear in two experimental conditions that were designed to produce similar trajectories of retinal image motion. In the fixation condition, a small bright target was presented for a duration of 50-200 ms in rightward or leftward motion, while the observer remained stationary and maintained fixation. In the VOR condition, the target moved along with the observer, who underwent full-body rotation around a vertical axis in darkness. Horizontal eye movement recordings during VOR trials allowed us to calculate the velocity of retinal image motion on each VOR trial. The principal result was that the extent of perceived motion smear was significantly less during VOR than fixation trials, particularly for target durations of 100 ms or longer. These findings support the conclusion that extra-retinal signals during the involuntary VOR contribute to a reduction of perceived motion smear.

[1]  A. A. Skavenski,et al.  Accuracy of eye position information for motor control , 1977, Vision Research.

[2]  H. Bedell,et al.  Perception of motion smear in normal observers and in persons with congenital nystagmus. , 1996, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[3]  Chris A. Johnson,et al.  THE PERCEIVED SIZE OF TARGETS IN THE PERIPHERAL AND CENTRAL VISUAL FIELDS , 1984, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[4]  L. Abel,et al.  Effects of stimulus size and luminance on oscillopsia in congenital nystagmus , 2003, Vision Research.

[5]  M Jeannerod,et al.  Oculomotor influences on lateral geniculate body neurons. , 1970, Brain research.

[6]  K Toyama,et al.  Integration of retinal and motor signals of eye movements in striate cortex cells of the alert cat. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  R. Leigh,et al.  Oscillopsia, retinal image stabilization and congenital nystagmus. , 1988, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[8]  V Di Lollo,et al.  Inverse-intensity effect in duration of visible persistence. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  Jean Lorenceau,et al.  The inverse intensity effect is not lost with stimuli in apparent motion , 1993, Vision Research.

[10]  R. Haber,et al.  Direct estimates of the apparent duration of a flash. , 1970 .

[11]  Jane H Sumnall,et al.  The extra-retinal motion aftereffect. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[12]  H. Leibowitz,et al.  A revised analysis of the role of efference in motion perception. , 1985, Perception.

[13]  John H Hogben,et al.  Suppression of visible persistence. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  F. Duffy,et al.  Eye movement-related inhibition of primate visual neurons , 1975, Brain Research.

[15]  K. Kawano,et al.  Response properties of neurons in posterior parietal cortex of monkey during visual-vestibular stimulation. I. Visual tracking neurons. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  B. Bridgeman A review of the role of efference copy in sensory and oculomotor control systems , 1995, Annals of Biomedical Engineering.

[17]  Avi Chaudhuri,et al.  A motion illusion generated by afternystagmus suppression , 1990, Neuroscience Letters.

[18]  Eric Castet,et al.  Effect of the ISI on the visible persistence of a stimulus in apparent motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[19]  B. Schneider,et al.  Changes in the Apparent Lengths of Lines as a Function of Degree of Retinal Eccentricity , 1978, Perception.

[20]  Richard V Abadi,et al.  Oscillopsia and tolerance to retinal image movement in congenital nystagmus. , 1999, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[21]  G. Geiger,et al.  Visual Recognition in the Peripheral Field: Letters versus Symbols and Adults versus Children , 1993, Perception.

[22]  R. B. Post,et al.  Induced motion and optokinetic afternystagmus: Parallel response dynamics with prolonged stimulation , 1988, Vision Research.

[23]  W. McDougall THE SENSATIONS EXCITED BY A SINGLE MOMENTARY STIMULATION OF THE EYE , 1904 .

[24]  Harold E Bedell,et al.  Suppression of motion-produced smear during smooth pursuit eye movements , 1996, Current Biology.

[25]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  Perception of a Clear and Stable Visual World with Congenital Nystagmus , 2000, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[26]  References , 1971 .

[27]  A. P. Mapp,et al.  A Restatement and Modification of Wells-Hering's Laws of Visual Direction , 1995, Perception.

[28]  S P McKee,et al.  Discrimination of time: comparison of foveal and peripheral sensitivity. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[29]  A. Bronstein,et al.  Vertical and horizontal motion perception in congenital nystagmus , 1998 .

[30]  C W Tyler,et al.  Analysis of visual modulation sensitivity. II. Peripheral retina and the role of photoreceptor dimensions. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[31]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  Asymmetry of perceived motion smear during head and eye movements: Evidence for a dichotomous neural categorization of retinal image motion , 2005, Vision Research.

[32]  G. Vanni-Mercier,et al.  Single neuron activity related to natural vestibular stimulation in the cat's visual cortex , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  V. Lollo,et al.  Suppression of visible persistence in apparent motion , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  A target in real motion appears blurred in the absence of other proximal moving targets , 1995, Vision Research.

[35]  A GRAYBIEL,et al.  VISUAL ILLUSIONS OF MOVEMENT. , 1965, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  P. Thier,et al.  Responses of Visual‐Tracking Neurons from Cortical Area MST‐I to Visual, Eye and Head Motion , 1992, The European journal of neuroscience.

[37]  Kikuro Fukushima,et al.  Pursuit-related neurons in the supplementary eye fields: discharge during pursuit and passive whole body rotation. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  H E Bedell,et al.  Extraretinal information about eye position during involuntary eye movement: Optokinetic afternystagmus , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  H. P. Goldstein,et al.  Visual remapping in infantile nystagmus , 1992, Vision Research.

[40]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  Attenuation of perceived motion smear during vergence and pursuit tracking , 2004, Vision Research.

[41]  A J Benson,et al.  Visual-vestibular interaction in the control of eye movement. , 1978, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[42]  H. Bedell,et al.  Extraretinal signals for congenital nystagmus. , 1993, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[43]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  The attenuation of perceived image smear during saccades , 2001, Vision Research.

[44]  M Fetter,et al.  Interaction of smooth pursuit and the vestibuloocular reflex in three dimensions. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[46]  J. Pola,et al.  Visual persistence: effects of flash luminance, duration and energy. , 1974, Vision research.

[47]  Shelford Bidwell Curiosities of Light and Sight , 1899, Nature.