Tracking Replicability as a Method of Post-Publication Open Evaluation
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Jacob Cohen,et al. THE STATISTICAL POWER OF ABNORMAL-SOCI AL PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH: , 1962 .
[2] J. Hirschhorn,et al. A comprehensive review of genetic association studies , 2002, Genetics in Medicine.
[3] D. Marx,et al. “Why Did I Get a ‘D’?” The Effects of Social Comparisons on Women’s Attributions to Discrimination , 2000 .
[4] Gerd Gigerenzer,et al. Do Studies of Statistical Power Have an Effect on the Power of Studies? , 2004 .
[5] Martin A. Lindquist,et al. Evaluating the consistency and specificity of neuroimaging data using meta-analysis , 2009, NeuroImage.
[6] K. Dickersin,et al. Factors influencing publication of research results. Follow-up of applications submitted to two institutional review boards. , 1992, JAMA.
[7] T. Braver,et al. Cognitive Neuroscience Approaches to Individual Differences in Working Memory and Executive Control: Conceptual and Methodological Issues , 2010 .
[8] Gerald Matthews,et al. Handbook of individual differences in cognition : attention, memory, and executive control , 2010 .
[9] M. Mahoney. Publication prejudices: An experimental study of confirmatory bias in the peer review system , 1977, Cognitive Therapy and Research.
[10] J. Ioannidis,et al. Establishment of genetic associations for complex diseases is independent of early study findings , 2004, European Journal of Human Genetics.
[11] R. Baayen,et al. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .
[12] Daniel E. Vetter,et al. Replication in strategic management: scientific testing for validity, generalizability, and usefulness , 1998 .
[13] J. Ioannidis,et al. Systematic Review of the Empirical Evidence of Study Publication Bias and Outcome Reporting Bias , 2008, PloS one.
[14] G. Taubes. Epidemiology faces its limits. , 1995, Science.
[15] H. Eysenck,et al. Peer review: Advice to referees and contributors , 1992 .
[16] T. Yarkoni. Big Correlations in Little Studies: Inflated fMRI Correlations Reflect Low Statistical Power—Commentary on Vul et al. (2009) , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[17] D. Goldstein,et al. UCHL‐1 is not a Parkinson's disease susceptibility gene , 2006, Annals of neurology.
[18] P. Armitage,et al. Repeated Significance Tests on Accumulating Data , 1969 .
[19] Paolo Boffetta,et al. False-Positive Results in Cancer Epidemiology: A Plea for Epistemological Modesty , 2008, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.
[20] G. Hardin,et al. The Tragedy of the Commons , 1968, Green Planet Blues.
[21] Petter Kristensen,et al. Explaining the Relation Between Birth Order and Intelligence , 2007, Science.
[22] Vikas Kumar,et al. CrowdSearch: exploiting crowds for accurate real-time image search on mobile phones , 2010, MobiSys '10.
[23] T. Jaeger,et al. Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.
[24] K M Ruggiero,et al. Less pain and more to gain: why high-status group members blame their failure on discrimination. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[25] J. Ioannidis. Contradicted and initially stronger effects in highly cited clinical research. , 2005, JAMA.
[26] M. Lindquist,et al. Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data: current and future directions. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[27] J. Giles. Internet encyclopaedias go head to head , 2005, Nature.
[28] Jeffrey Harris,et al. Why children turn out the way they do , 1998 .
[29] M. Khoury,et al. Most Published Research Findings Are False—But a Little Replication Goes a Long Way , 2007, PLoS medicine.
[30] Jonathan Flint,et al. Replication and heterogeneity in gene x environment interaction studies. , 2009, The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology.
[31] Tim Kraska,et al. CrowdDB: answering queries with crowdsourcing , 2011, SIGMOD '11.
[32] Marian Perkins,et al. The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do , 2000, BMJ : British Medical Journal.
[33] M. Munafo,et al. Bias in genetic association studies and impact factor , 2009, Molecular Psychiatry.
[34] G. Gigerenzer,et al. Do studies of statistical power have an effect on the power of studies , 1989 .
[35] Eleanor Singer,et al. Birth Order: Its Influence on Personality , 1983 .
[36] George Dunea. Too many to count , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.
[37] Joshua K. Hartshorne,et al. Birth order effects in the formation of long-term relationships , 2009 .
[38] L. Bero,et al. Publication bias and research on passive smoking: comparison of published and unpublished studies. , 1998, JAMA.
[39] Thomas A Trikalinos,et al. Genetic associations in large versus small studies: an empirical assessment , 2003, The Lancet.
[40] D. Howells,et al. Publication Bias in Reports of Animal Stroke Studies Leads to Major Overstatement of Efficacy , 2010, PLoS biology.
[41] P. Easterbrook,et al. Publication bias in clinical research , 1991, The Lancet.
[42] J. Ioannidis. Why Most Published Research Findings Are False , 2005, PLoS medicine.
[43] Franz Porzsolt,et al. The fading of reported effectiveness. A meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials , 2006, BMC medical research methodology.
[44] Chris Callison-Burch,et al. Fast, Cheap, and Creative: Evaluating Translation Quality Using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk , 2009, EMNLP.
[45] Jie W Weiss,et al. Bayesian Statistical Inference for Psychological Research , 2008 .
[46] Laura A. Dabbish,et al. Designing games with a purpose , 2008, CACM.
[47] C. F. Bond,et al. One Hundred Years of Social Psychology Quantitatively Described , 2003 .
[48] P. Albert,et al. A Cautionary Note on the Robustness of Latent Class Models for Estimating Diagnostic Error without a Gold Standard , 2004, Biometrics.
[49] Timothy D. Evans,et al. Journal of Individual Psychology , 1989 .
[50] S. Ceci,et al. Peer-review practices of psychological journals: The fate of published articles, submitted again , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[51] J. Ioannidis,et al. Replication validity of genetic association studies , 2001, Nature Genetics.
[52] Edwin E. Wagner,et al. Effect of positive findings on submission and acceptance rates: A note on meta-analysis bias. , 1986 .
[53] Alon Y. Halevy,et al. Crowdsourcing systems on the World-Wide Web , 2011, Commun. ACM.
[54] L. J. Chase,et al. A statistical power analysis of applied psychological research. , 1976 .
[55] Mark A. Mone,et al. THE PERCEPTIONS AND USAGE OF STATISTICAL POWER IN APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY AND MANAGEMENT RESEARCH , 1996 .
[56] Brendan T. O'Connor,et al. Cheap and Fast – But is it Good? Evaluating Non-Expert Annotations for Natural Language Tasks , 2008, EMNLP.
[57] Roger B. Dannenberg,et al. TagATune: A Game for Music and Sound Annotation , 2007, ISMIR.
[58] Matt Field,et al. A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.
[59] H. Pashler,et al. Puzzlingly High Correlations in fMRI Studies of Emotion, Personality, and Social Cognition 1 , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[60] E. Lander,et al. Meta-analysis of genetic association studies supports a contribution of common variants to susceptibility to common disease , 2003, Nature Genetics.
[61] J. R. Cole,et al. Chance and consensus in peer review. , 1981, Science.
[62] Benjamin B. Bederson,et al. Translation by iterative collaboration between monolingual users , 2010, HCOMP '10.
[63] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. Divide and conquer: A defense of functional localizers , 2006, NeuroImage.
[64] A. Tversky,et al. BELIEF IN THE LAW OF SMALL NUMBERS , 1971, Pediatrics.
[65] D. Rennie,et al. Publication bias in editorial decision making. , 2002, JAMA.
[66] Jacob Cohen,et al. The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research: a review. , 1962, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.
[67] D. Goldstein,et al. UCHL1 is a Parkinson's disease susceptibility gene. , 2004 .
[68] W. K. Simmons,et al. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.
[69] A. MØller,et al. Publication bias in ecology and evolution: an empirical assessment using the ‘trim and fill’ method , 2002, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[70] Donald P. Green,et al. Testing for Publication Bias in Political Science , 2001, Political Analysis.
[71] Douglas P. Newton,et al. Quality and Peer Review of Research: An Adjudicating Role for Editors , 2010, Accountability in research.
[72] Jerry G. Thursby,et al. Replication in Empirical Economics: The Journal of Money, Credit and Banking Project , 1986 .
[73] Roxana M Gonzalez,et al. Retraction: Forecasting One’s Future Based on Fleeting Subjective Experiences , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[74] Manuel Blum,et al. reCAPTCHA: Human-Based Character Recognition via Web Security Measures , 2008, Science.
[75] K. Dickersin,et al. Publication bias and clinical trials. , 1987, Controlled clinical trials.
[76] Edna Mora Szymanski,et al. Statistical power analysis of rehabilitation counseling research. , 1993 .
[77] R. Wears,et al. Positive-outcome bias and other limitations in the outcome of research abstracts submitted to a scientific meeting. , 1998, JAMA.
[78] John P A Ioannidis,et al. Meta‐research: The art of getting it wrong , 2010, Research synthesis methods.
[79] M. Jennions,et al. Relationships fade with time: a meta-analysis of temporal trends in publication in ecology and evolution , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[80] Lada A. Adamic,et al. Knowledge sharing and yahoo answers: everyone knows something , 2008, WWW.
[81] C. Ferguson,et al. Much ado about nothing: the misestimation and overinterpretation of violent video game effects in eastern and western nations: comment on Anderson et al. (2010). , 2010, Psychological bulletin.
[82] George Davey Smith,et al. Meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials , 1997, The Lancet.
[83] E. Wagenmakers,et al. Why psychologists must change the way they analyze their data: the case of psi: comment on Bem (2011). , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[84] Winny Shen,et al. Samples in applied psychology: over a decade of research in review. , 2011, The Journal of applied psychology.
[85] R. Graves,et al. Statistical Power and Effect Sizes of Clinical Neuropsychology Research , 2001, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.
[86] Gerhard Andersson,et al. Efficacy of cognitive–behavioural therapy and other psychological treatments for adult depression: meta-analytic study of publication bias , 2010, British Journal of Psychiatry.