Modeling memory decline in older adults: the importance of preclinical dementia, attrition, and chronological age.

This longitudinal study examined memory loss in a sample of 391 initially nondemented older adults. Analyses decomposed observed memory loss into decline associated with preclinical dementia, study attrition, terminal decline, and chronological age. Measuring memory as a function of only chronological age failed to provide an adequate representation of cognitive change. Disease progression accounted for virtually all of the memory loss in the 25% of the sample that developed diagnosable dementia. In the remainder of the sample, both chronological age and study attrition contributed to observed memory loss. These results suggest that much of memory loss in aging adults may be attributable to the progression of preclinical dementia and other nonnormative aging processes that are not captured by chronological age.

[1]  K. Schaie,et al.  The relationship between prior functioning on cognitive and personality dimensions and subject attrition in longitudinal research. , 1988, Journal of gerontology.

[2]  J. Birren,et al.  Research on the psychology of aging: Principles, concepts and theory. , 1985 .

[3]  L. Fratiglioni,et al.  Stability of the preclinical episodic memory deficit in Alzheimer's disease. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  P. Baltes,et al.  Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: a new window to the study of cognitive aging? , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[5]  H. Buschke,et al.  The effects of preclinical dementia on estimates of normal cognitive functioning in aging. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[6]  Claudia H. Kawas,et al.  The epidemiology of dementia and Alzheimer disease. , 1994 .

[7]  M. Roth,et al.  The Association Between Quantitative Measures of Dementia and of Senile Change in the Cerebral Grey Matter of Elderly Subjects , 1968, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[8]  James E. Birren,et al.  Handbook of Aging and the Individual , 1961 .

[9]  J. Birren,et al.  History, concepts, and theory in the psychology of aging , 1996 .

[10]  J. Ware,et al.  Random-effects models for longitudinal data. , 1982, Biometrics.

[11]  J. G. Evans Ageing and disease. , 1988, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[12]  J. Schneider,et al.  Individual differences in rates of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[13]  J. Birren,et al.  Handbook of the psychology of aging, 3rd ed. , 1985 .

[14]  R. Katzman.,et al.  Clinical, pathological, and neurochemical changes in dementia: A subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques , 1988, Annals of neurology.

[15]  R. Dixon,et al.  Short-term longitudinal change in cognitive performance in later life. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[16]  Joachim F. Wohlwill,et al.  The age variable in psychological research. , 1970 .

[17]  T. Salthouse The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.

[18]  R. Katzman.,et al.  Alzheimer's disease : senile dementia and related disorders , 1978 .

[19]  Paul B. Baltes,et al.  Intellectual Development in Adulthood: The Seattle Longitudinal Study , 1996 .

[20]  Klaus F. Riegel,et al.  A study of the dropout rates in longitudinal research on aging and the prediction of death. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  J. Miller,et al.  A prospective study of cognitive function and onset of dementia in cognitively healthy elders. , 1998, Archives of neurology.

[22]  M. Rockstein,et al.  Biology of Aging , 1958 .

[23]  E. Metter,et al.  Next steps in describing aging and disease in longitudinal studies. , 1990, Journal of gerontology.

[24]  H. Christensen,et al.  An analysis of diversity in the cognitive performance of elderly community dwellers: individual differences in change scores as a function of age. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[25]  S. Raudenbush,et al.  Comparing personal trajectories and drawing causal inferences from longitudinal data. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.

[26]  L. Jarvik,et al.  Cognitive Function and Prediction of Dementia in Old Age , 1987, International journal of aging & human development.

[27]  B. Winblad,et al.  The course of cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer disease: three- and 6-year follow-up of a population-based sample. , 2000, Archives of neurology.

[28]  G Ratcliff,et al.  Patterns of cognitive decline in presymptomatic Alzheimer disease: a prospective community study. , 2001, Archives of general psychiatry.

[29]  K. Marder,et al.  Neuropsychological detection and characterization of preclinical Alzheimer's disease , 1995, Neurology.

[30]  A. Yashin,et al.  Heterogeneity's ruses: some surprising effects of selection on population dynamics. , 1985, The American statistician.

[31]  James S. Goodwin Geriatric Ideology: The Myth of the Myth of Senility , 1991, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[32]  C B Hall,et al.  A change point model for estimating the onset of cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. , 2000, Statistics in medicine.

[33]  M N Rossor,et al.  Presymptomatic cognitive deficits in individuals at risk of familial Alzheimer's disease. A longitudinal prospective study. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  D. Jolley,et al.  Cost benefits of a medically supervised day treatment program for patients with Alzheimer's disease and other forms of dementia , 1989 .

[35]  W. Jagust,et al.  The Role of APOE ∊4 in Modulating Effects of Other Risk Factors for Cognitive Decline in Elderly Persons , 1999 .

[36]  Herman Buschke,et al.  Selective reminding for analysis of memory and learning , 1973 .

[37]  A. Korten,et al.  The prevalence of dementia: A quantitative integration of the literature , 1987, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[38]  John L. Horn,et al.  Major abilities and development in the adult period. , 1992 .

[39]  D. Hultsch,et al.  Memory Change in the Aged , 1998 .

[40]  S. Mann,et al.  Ciba Foundation Symposium , 1997 .

[41]  Robin J. Caster Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, 5th ed. , 2003 .

[42]  John R. Nesselroade,et al.  History and rationale of longitudinal research , 1979 .

[43]  加藤 雅啓 Douglas J. FuTUYMA著, Evolutionary Biology. 2nd ed., xii+600頁, Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Massachusetts, ISBN 0-87893-188-0 , 1987 .

[44]  Ewald W. Busse,et al.  Behavior and Adaptation in Late Life , 1971 .

[45]  L. Bäckman,et al.  Time to Death and Cognitive Performance , 1999 .

[46]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  Subject attrition in a longitudinal study of cognitive performance in community-based elderly people. , 1994 .

[47]  S. Berg Aging, behavior and terminal decline , 1996 .

[48]  K. Schaie,et al.  Generational and Cohort-Specific Differences in Adult Cognitive Functioning: A Fourteen-Year Study of Independent Samples. , 1973 .

[49]  Roberta F. White,et al.  The preclinical phase of alzheimer disease: A 22-year prospective study of the Framingham Cohort. , 2000, Archives of neurology.

[50]  R. Michels,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd ed , 1981 .

[51]  John R. Nesselroade,et al.  Longitudinal Research in the Study of Behavior and Development , 1979 .

[52]  J J Brace,et al.  Theories of aging: an overview. , 1981 .

[53]  R. Holliday Understanding Ageing: Author index , 1995 .

[54]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Handbook of the Psychology of Aging , 1979 .

[55]  H. Buschke,et al.  Cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships among age, cognition, and processing speed. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[56]  C B Hall,et al.  Estimation of bivariate measurements having different change points, with application to cognitive ageing , 2001, Statistics in medicine.