P300 Habituation Patterns: Individual Differences from Ultradian Rhythms

Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were elicited with auditory stimuli from normal subjects every 10 min. for 10 successive trial blocks to assess ultradian influences on the P300 component. Based on a theoretical analysis of the underlying relationship between background EEG activity and P300 variability, subjects were divided into two groups according to whether P300 amplitude increased or decreased initially over trial blocks. P300 amplitude habituated across trial blocks. P300 latency produced systematic cyclical variation with approximately a 90-min period that was opposite in phase for the different subject groups. The findings suggest that ultradian rhythms contribute to P300 latency variability. The implications o the results for P300 in applied settings are discussed.

[1]  Gavin S. Lew,et al.  P300, habituation, and response mode , 1993, Physiology & Behavior.

[2]  F E Bloom,et al.  P300 assessment of early Alzheimer's disease. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[3]  N. Squires,et al.  Active and passive P3 latency in dementia: Relationship to psychometric, electroencephalographic, and computed tomographic measures , 1990 .

[4]  K. Spencer,et al.  Poststimulus EEG spectral analysis and P300: attention, task, and probability. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[5]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  The reliability of ERP components in the auditory oddball paradigm. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[6]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[7]  J. Polich,et al.  P300, food consumption, and memory performance. , 1992, Psychophysiology.

[8]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 and time of day: Circadian rhythms, food intake, and body temperature , 1990, Biological Psychology.

[9]  D. Goodin,et al.  Effect of Different High‐Pass Filters on the Long‐Latency Event‐Related Auditory Evoked Potentials in Normal Human Subjects and Individuals Infected with the Human Immunodeficiency Virus , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology.

[10]  Werner Sommer,et al.  Consciousness of attention and expectancy as reflected in event-related potentials and reaction times , 1990 .

[11]  J. Polich,et al.  Normal variation of P300 in children: age, memory span, and head size. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[12]  Peretz Lavie,et al.  Ultradian rhythms: gates of sleep and wakefulness , 1985 .

[13]  J. Polich,et al.  P300, cognitive capability, and personality: A correlational study of university undergraduates , 1992 .

[14]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 and individual differences: morning/evening activity preference, food, and time-of-day. , 1992, Psychophysiology.

[15]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 development from auditory stimuli. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[16]  M. Treisman Temporal Rhythms and Cerebral Rhythms a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[17]  W. van Winsum,et al.  The functional significance of event-related desynchronization of alpha rhythm in attentional and activating tasks. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[18]  R. Broughton,et al.  Bilaterally synchronous ultradian EEG rhythms in awake adult humans. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[19]  D. Stoffer,et al.  Ultradian rhythms in performance on tests of specialized cognitive function. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.

[20]  J. Polich,et al.  Normal variation of P300 from auditory stimuli. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[21]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Sleepiness on the job: continuously measured EEG changes in train drivers. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[22]  N. Squires,et al.  Evoked potential changes and neuropsychological performance in Parkinson's disease , 1987, Biological Psychology.

[23]  S. Ortega,et al.  Ultradian rhythms in the EEG and task performance. , 1990 .

[24]  J. Polich On the correlation between P300 amplitude and latency , 1992 .

[25]  W. Klimesch EEG-alpha rhythms and memory processes. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[26]  P. Lavie,et al.  Biological rhythms in arousal indices: a potential confounding effect in EEG biofeedback. , 1983, Psychophysiology.

[27]  J. Intriligator,et al.  On the relationship between background EEG and the P300 event-related potential , 1994, Biological Psychology.

[28]  T Kobayashi,et al.  Short and long ultradian EEG components in daytime arousal. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[29]  D. Lloyd,et al.  THE OCCURRENCE AND FUNCTIONS OF ULTRADIAN RHYTHMS , 1991, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[30]  G. Pfurtscheller Event-related synchronization (ERS): an electrophysiological correlate of cortical areas at rest. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[31]  J. Polich P300 clinical utility and control of variability. , 1998, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[32]  R. Emmerson,et al.  P3 latency and symbol digit performance correlations in aging. , 1989, Experimental aging research.

[33]  J. Intriligator,et al.  On the relationship between EEG and ERP variability. , 1995, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[34]  R. Klein,et al.  Rhythms in human performance: 1 1/2-hour oscillations in cognitive style. , 1979, Science.

[35]  John Polich,et al.  P3(00) habituation from auditory and visual stimuli , 1996, Physiology & Behavior.

[36]  Erol Başar,et al.  The EEG is a Quasi-Deterministic Signal Anticipating Sensory-Cognitive Tasks , 1989 .

[37]  E. Donchin,et al.  Cognitive Psychophysiology and Human Information Processing , 1986 .

[38]  F. Bloom,et al.  P300 from an auditory oddball task: inter-laboratory consistency. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[39]  E. Donchin,et al.  Performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. , 1983, Science.

[40]  E Başar,et al.  A new approach to endogenous event-related potentials in man: relation between EEG and P300-wave. , 1984, The International journal of neuroscience.

[41]  G. Hakerem,et al.  The effect of prestimulus alpha activity on the P300. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[42]  J. Polich,et al.  Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[43]  Gerard A. Kerkhof,et al.  Inter-individual differences in the human circadian system: A review , 1985, Biological Psychology.

[44]  W. van Winsum,et al.  Event-related desynchronization and P300. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[45]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Alpha frequency and memory performance , 1990 .

[46]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Event related potentials and EEG components in a semantic memory search task. , 1992, Psychophysiology.

[47]  J. Polich,et al.  On the relationship between EEG and P300: individual differences, aging, and ultradian rhythms. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[48]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Event-related cortical desynchronization detected by power measurements of scalp EEG. , 1977, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[49]  F. Boiten,et al.  Event-related desynchronization: the effects of energetic and computational demands. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[50]  P. Robaey,et al.  A topographical ERP study of healthy premature 5-year-old children in the auditory and visual modalities. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[51]  G Pfurtscheller,et al.  Graphical display and statistical evaluation of event-related desynchronization (ERD). , 1977, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[52]  P. Badia,et al.  Habituation of P300 to target stimuli , 1989, Physiology & Behavior.

[53]  M. Matousek,et al.  Spontaneous vigilance fluctuations in the daytime. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[54]  P. Badia,et al.  Time of day and semantic category effects on late components of the visual ERP , 1992, Biological Psychology.

[55]  D. Shannahoff-Khalsa,et al.  Ultradian rhythms of autonomic, cardiovascular, and neuroendocrine systems are related in humans. , 1996, The American journal of physiology.

[56]  J. Polich Habituation of P300 from auditory stimuli , 1989, Psychobiology.