Mnemonic Function of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Conflict-Induced Behavioral Adjustment
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[2] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning , 2004, Brain and Cognition.
[3] Elisa Ciaramelli,et al. The Regulation of Cognitive Control following Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex Lesion in Humans , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[4] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[5] Colin M. Macleod,et al. Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[6] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Prefrontal Cell Activities Related to Monkeys' Success and Failure in Adapting to Rule Changes in a Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Analog , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[7] C. Liston,et al. Anterior Cingulate and Posterior Parietal Cortices Are Sensitive to Dissociable Forms of Conflict in a Task-Switching Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.
[8] D. Stuss,et al. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[9] H. E. Rosvold,et al. Analysis of the delayed-alternation deficit produced by dorsolateral prefrontal lesions in the rhesus monkey. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[10] T. Robbins,et al. A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[11] T. Egner,et al. Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[12] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Behavioral evidence for working memory of sensory dimension in macaque monkeys , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.
[13] Joshua W. Brown,et al. Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes , 2002, Neuron.
[14] K Tsutsui,et al. Interference from irrelevant features on visual discrimination by macaques (Macaca fuscata): a behavioral analogue of the human Stroop effect. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[15] Jin Fan,et al. Cognitive and Brain Consequences of Conflict , 2003, NeuroImage.
[16] P S Goldman-Rakic,et al. Association of Storage and Processing Functions in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex of the Nonhuman Primate , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[17] M. Farah,et al. Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control? , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[18] E. Miller,et al. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.
[19] M. Roesch,et al. Neuronal activity in macaque SEF and ACC during performance of tasks involving conflict. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[20] A. Turken,et al. Dissociation between conflict detection and error monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[21] C. Carter,et al. The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[22] M. Petrides. The role of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in working memory , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[23] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.
[24] R. Knight,et al. Prefrontal–cingulate interactions in action monitoring , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[25] Tobias Egner,et al. The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task , 2005, NeuroImage.