The relation between social rank, neophobia and individual learning in starlings

Researchers with diverse interests in topics ranging from the formation of dominance hierarchies and social intelligence to animal personalities have predicted specific, and often conflicting, relations between social rank, neophobia and learning ability. We investigated the relations between these variables in captive groups of wild-caught starlings, Sturnus vulgaris, adopting a multidimensional approach to social rank and neophobia. Both agonistic and competitive rank orders were determined for each group and we tested individuals in the absence of their groupmates for object neophobia, latency to feed in a novel environment and performance on an extractive foraging task. In each starling group, the fastest learners occupied the highest competitive ranks, supporting the hypothesis that cognitive ability is positively correlated with social dominance. Competitive rank orders, however, did not correlate significantly with agonistic rank orders. Situation-specific foraging neophobia was suggested: individuals showed consistency in their latencies to feed near a variety of novel objects, but no significant correlation was found between this measure of object neophobia and latency to feed in a novel environment. Starlings fastest to feed in the novel environment were fastest in solving the foraging task. We discuss the implications of these findings for researchers studying hierarchy formation in animal groups, social intelligence and animal personalities.

[1]  G. Katzir Relationships Between Social Structure and Response To Novelty in Captive Jackdaws, Corvus Monedula L. Ii. Response To Novel Palatable Food , 1982 .

[2]  R. Greenberg Differences in feeding neophobia in the tropical migrant wood warblers Dendroica castanea and D. pensylvanica. , 1984 .

[3]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[4]  W. Rice ANALYZING TABLES OF STATISTICAL TESTS , 1989, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[5]  J. Cambefort A comparative study of culturally transmitted patterns of feeding habits in the chacma baboon Papio ursinus and the vervet monkey Cercopithecus aethiops. , 1981, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[6]  C. Barnard,et al.  Acquisition of dominance status affects maze learning in mice , 2002, Behavioural Processes.

[7]  G. Syme,et al.  An analysis of the limited access measure of social dominance in rats , 1974 .

[8]  A. Goldsmith,et al.  Social stimulation and regulation of body mass in female starlings , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Differences in Individual Learning Between Group-Foraging and Territorial Zenaida Doves , 1996 .

[10]  B. N. Bunnell,et al.  Performance correlates of social behavior and organization: Social rank and complex problem solving in crab-eating macaques (M. fascicularis) , 1980, Primates.

[11]  R. Greenberg The Role of Neophobia in Determining the Degree of Foraging Specialization in Some Migrant Warblers , 1983, The American Naturalist.

[12]  C. Heyes,et al.  Social learning in animals : the roots of culture , 1996 .

[13]  J. Langbein,et al.  Analysing dominance relationships by sociometric methods—a plea for a more standardised and precise approach in farm animals , 2004 .

[14]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Public information cues affect the scrounging decisions of starlings , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  C. Feare,et al.  Differences in the ages, sexes and physical condition of Starlings Sturnus vulgaris at the centre and periphery of roosts , 1987 .

[16]  R. C. Newberry,et al.  Comparison of social ranks based on worm-running and aggressive behaviour in young domestic fowl , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[17]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  What are big brains for? , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  De,et al.  Finding a dominance order most consistent with a linear hierarchy: a new procedure and review. , 1998, Animal behaviour.

[19]  George V. N. Powell,et al.  Experimental analysis of the social value of flocking by starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) in relation to predation and foraging , 1974 .

[20]  Alex Kacelnik,et al.  Increasing the costs of conspecific scanning in socially foraging starlings affects vigilance and foraging behaviour , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  C. Boesch,et al.  Food Competition and Linear Dominance Hierarchy Among Female Chimpanzees of the Taï National Park , 2003, International Journal of Primatology.

[22]  J. Templeton,et al.  Sociality and social learning in two species of corvids: the pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) and the Clark's nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana). , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.

[23]  M. G. King Disruptions in the pecking order of cockerels concomitant with degrees of accessability to feed. , 1965, Animal behaviour.

[24]  H. Vries,et al.  AN EXPERIMENTAL EVALUATION OF THE CONSISTENCY OF COMPETITIVE ABILITY AND AGONISTIC DOMINANCE IN DIFFERENT SOCIAL CONTEXTS IN CAPTIVE BONOBOS , 1999 .

[25]  H. de Vries,et al.  Dominance and its Behavioral Measures in a Captive Group of Bonobos (Pan paniscus) , 2000, International Journal of Primatology.

[26]  T. Bugnyar,et al.  Leading a conspecific away from food in ravens (Corvus corax)? , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[27]  J. Swaddle,et al.  Food, feathers and fluctuating asymmetries , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  S. M. Richards The concept of dominance and methods of assessment , 1974 .

[29]  I. Chase,et al.  Behavioral Sequences During Dominance Hierarchy Formation in Chickens , 1982, Science.

[30]  K. Laland,et al.  Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  F. James Rohlf,et al.  Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Research , 1969 .

[32]  H. Kummer,et al.  Conditions of Innovative Behaviour in Primates , 1985 .

[33]  Andrew Sih,et al.  Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. , 2004, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[34]  Paul S. Martin,et al.  Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide , 1986 .

[35]  Han de Vries,et al.  David's score: a more appropriate dominance ranking method than Clutton-Brock et al.'s index , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[36]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Cost can increase preference in starlings , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[37]  B. N. Bunnell,et al.  Performance correlates of social behavior and organization: Social rank and reversal learning in crab-eating macaques (M. fascicularis) , 1980, Primates.

[38]  G. Syme Competitive orders as measures of social dominance , 1974 .

[39]  Philip N. Lehner,et al.  Handbook of ethological methods , 1979 .

[40]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Problem solving and neophobia in a columbiform–passeriform assemblage in Barbados , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[41]  K. Laland,et al.  Primate Innovation: Sex, Age and Social Rank Differences , 2001, International Journal of Primatology.

[42]  I. Chase,et al.  Social Process and Hierarchy Formation in Small Groups: A Comparative Perspective , 1980 .

[43]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Accuracy of memory for amount in the foraging starling, Sturnus vulgaris , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[44]  C. Heyes,et al.  Stimulus learning and response learning by observation in the European starling, in a two-object/two-action test , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[45]  T. W. Fawcett,et al.  A test of imitative learning in starlings using a two-action method with an enhanced ghost control , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[46]  D. Wilson,et al.  Shyness and boldness in pumpkinseed sunfish: individual differences are context-specific , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[47]  J. Templeton Learning from others' mistakes: a paradox revisited , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[48]  J. Swaddle,et al.  Chest plumage, dominance and fluctuating asymmetry in female starlings , 1995, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[49]  R. Hinde,et al.  Growing Points in Ethology , 1976 .

[50]  J. Swaddle,et al.  Fluctuating asymmetries, competition and dominance , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[51]  Paul Martin,et al.  Measuring behaviour: An introductory guide, 2nd ed. , 1993 .

[52]  T. Caraco,et al.  Social Foraging Theory , 2018 .

[53]  Ivan D. Chase,et al.  Models of hierarchy formation in animal societies , 1974 .

[54]  K. Laland,et al.  Animal innovation: An introduction. , 2003 .

[55]  R. Greenberg The role of neophobia and neophilia in the development of innovative behaviour of birds , 2003 .

[56]  K. Wallen,et al.  Low-status monkeys "play dumb" when learning in mixed social groups. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[57]  J. Stahl,et al.  Subordinates explore but dominants profit: resource competition in high Arctic barnacle goose flocks , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[58]  Kees van Oers,et al.  Realized heritability of personalities in the great tit (Parus major) , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[59]  C. Nicol,et al.  The effects of demonstrator social status and prior foraging success on social learning in laying hens , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[60]  Craig Tovey,et al.  Individual differences versus social dynamics in the formation of animal dominance hierarchies , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[61]  Louis Lefebvre,et al.  CHAPTER 6 – Is Social Learning an Adaptive Specialization? , 1996 .

[62]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Learning Differences between Feral Pigeons and Zenaida Doves: The Role of Neophobia and Human Proximity , 2001 .

[63]  C. Janson,et al.  Social foraging and the finder's share in capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[64]  K. Laland,et al.  Foraging innovation in the guppy , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[65]  N. Humphrey The Social Function of Intellect , 1976 .

[66]  R. Byrne,et al.  Machiavellian intelligence : social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans , 1990 .

[67]  Hilde Vervaecke,et al.  Measuring and testing the steepness of dominance hierarchies , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[68]  S. Tebbich,et al.  Social manipulation causes cooperation in keas , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[69]  Robin I. M. Dunbar The Social Brain: Mind, Language, and Society in Evolutionary Perspective , 2003 .