The maturation of form and motion perception in school age children

The purpose of the current study was to investigate the maturation of form and motion perception, specifically the component visual abilities involved in the identification of motion-defined form, in children ranging in age from 3 to 12 years. Experiment 1 compared the maturation of motion-defined and texture-defined shape identification. Minimum speed thresholds on the motion-defined shape task decreased until age 7 years. Orientation difference thresholds on the texture-defined shape task decreased until age 11 years. Experiment 2 compared the maturation of global motion and global texture direction discrimination. Coherence thresholds on both tasks were similar in children of all ages and adults. Experiment 3 compared the maturation of direction discrimination on motion coherence and motion displacement tasks. Maximum displacement thresholds (Dmax) increased until age 7 years. The results are discussed with respect to the maturation of M/dorsal and P/ventral visual pathways.

[1]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Dorsal and ventral stream sensitivity in normal development and hemiplegia , 2002, Neuroreport.

[2]  M. J. Morgan,et al.  Spatial filtering precedes motion detection , 1992, Nature.

[3]  Tom Banton,et al.  Infants’ sensitivity to statistical distributions of motion direction and speed , 1999, Vision Research.

[4]  P. Rakić,et al.  Early divergence of magnocellular and parvocellular functional subsystems in the embryonic primate visual system. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Role of the color-opponent and broad-band channels in vision , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.

[6]  J. Stein,et al.  Contrast sensitivity and coherent motion detection measured at photopic luminance levels in dyslexics and controls , 1995, Vision Research.

[7]  D. C. Van Essen,et al.  Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex , 1988, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  H Strasburger,et al.  Converting between measures of slope of the psychometric function , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  J. Raymond,et al.  Visual Motion Perception in Children with Dyslexia: Normal Detection but Abnormal Integration , 1998 .

[10]  D. Regan,et al.  Visual processing of motion-defined form: selective failure in patients with parietotemporal lesions , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  D. Heeger,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Early Visual Pathways in Dyslexia , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Visual segmentation of oriented textures by infants , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  R. Kaufmann-Hayoz,et al.  Kinetic contours in infants' visual perception. , 1986, Child development.

[14]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[15]  D Regan,et al.  Development of Motion-Defined Figure-Ground Segregation in Preschool and Older Children, Using a Letter-Identification Task , 1997, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[16]  R. W. Rodieck,et al.  Retinal ganglion cell classes in the Old World monkey: morphology and central projections. , 1981, Science.

[17]  T. L. Hickey,et al.  Postnatal development of the human lateral geniculate nucleus: relationship to a critical period for the visual system , 1977, Science.

[18]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  D. Burr,et al.  Two stages of visual processing for radial and circular motion , 1995, Nature.

[20]  R. Turner,et al.  Form and motion coherence activate independent, but not dorsal/ventral segregated, networks in the human brain , 2000, Current Biology.

[21]  J. Movshon,et al.  Analysis of the development of spatial contrast sensitivity in monkey and human infants. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[22]  JH Maunsell,et al.  Does primate motion perception depend on the magnocellular pathway? , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  D. Regan,et al.  Reversible dissociation of sensitivity to dynamic stimuli in Parkinson's disease: Is magnocellular function essential to reading motion-defined letters? , 1997, Vision Research.

[24]  John Wattam-Bell,et al.  Coherence thresholds for discrimination of motion direction in infants , 1994, Vision Research.

[25]  O. Braddick,et al.  Masking of low frequency information in short-range apparent motion , 1990, Vision Research.

[26]  Julian Lewis Programs of cell division in the lateral line system , 1986, Trends in Neurosciences.

[27]  R. Campbell,et al.  High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. , 2002, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[28]  Szabolcs Kéri,et al.  [Parallel processing of visual information]. , 2006, Ideggyogyaszati szemle.

[29]  S. Watamaniuk High-Level Motion Processing: Computational, Neurobiological, and Psychophysical Perspectives , 1999 .

[30]  V. Lollo,et al.  Perception of directional sampled motion in relation to displacement and spatial frequency: Evidence for a unitary motion system , 1990, Vision Research.

[31]  Daphne L. McCulloch,et al.  A VEP investigation of parallel visual pathway development in primary school age children , 2004, Documenta Ophthalmologica.

[32]  R. Fox,et al.  Stereoacuity in young children. , 1986, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[33]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[34]  J Wattam-Bell,et al.  Visual motion processing in one-month-old infants: Preferential looking experiments , 1996, Vision Research.

[35]  T. Papathomas Early vision and beyond , 1995 .

[36]  Visual processing in the primate extrastriate cortex , 1995 .

[37]  A. Watson Probability summation over time , 1979, Vision Research.

[38]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  How parallel are the primate visual pathways? , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[39]  L. Spillmann,et al.  Visual Perception: The Neurophysiological Foundations , 1989 .

[40]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  High-Level Motion Processing , 1998 .

[41]  D. Regan Low-contrast visual acuity test for pediatric use. , 1988, Canadian journal of ophthalmology. Journal canadien d'ophtalmologie.

[42]  G A Orban,et al.  Discrimination of opposite directions measured with stroboscopically illuminated random-dot patterns. , 1989, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[43]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral visual pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Peter Lennie,et al.  6 – PARALLEL PROCESSING OF VISUAL INFORMATION , 1989 .

[45]  Robert F Dougherty,et al.  Psychophysical Indexes of Temporal Processing Abnormalities in Children With Developmental Dyslexia , 2004, Developmental neuropsychology.

[46]  John Wattam-Bell,et al.  The development of maximum displacement limits for discrimination of motion direction in infancy , 1992, Vision Research.

[47]  T. Buckingham,et al.  The Comparative Development of Movement Hyperacuity and Visual Acuity in Children , 1996, Vision Research.

[48]  R. Shapley,et al.  Cat and monkey retinal ganglion cells and their visual functional roles , 1986, Trends in Neurosciences.

[49]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[50]  D. Regan,et al.  Defective processing of motion-defined form in the fellow eye of patients with unilateral amblyopia. , 1992, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[51]  Ruxandra Sireteanu,et al.  Texture segregation in infants and children , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[52]  P. Rakic,et al.  Development of visual centers in the primate brain depends on binocular competition before birth. , 1981, Science.

[53]  Manfred Fahle,et al.  The development of vernier acuity in human infants , 1991, Vision Research.

[54]  Jan M. Ruijter,et al.  Visual half-field development in children: detection of motion-defined forms , 1998, Vision Research.

[55]  Paul V McGraw,et al.  Deficits to global motion processing in human amblyopia , 2003, Vision Research.

[56]  J. Maunsell,et al.  The effects of parvocellular lateral geniculate lesions on the acuity and contrast sensitivity of macaque monkeys , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[57]  J T Holladay,et al.  Proper method for calculating average visual acuity. , 1997, Journal of refractive surgery.

[58]  R. Fox,et al.  Mental retardation and perception of global motion , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[59]  B. Julesz,et al.  Displacement limits, directional anisotropy and direction versus form discrimination in random-dot cinematograms , 1983, Vision Research.

[60]  Walter H. Ehrenstein,et al.  Development of dynamic vision based on motion contrast , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.