Motion-induced position shifts occur after motion integration

Low-level motion processing in the primate visual system involves two stages. The first stage (in V1) contains specialised motion sensors which respond to local retinal motion, and the second stage (in MT) pools local signals to encode rigid surface motion. Recent psychophysical research shows that motion signals influence the perceived position of an object (motion-induced position shift, MIPS). In the present paper we investigate the role played by the two processing stages in generating MIPS. We compared MIPS induced by single grating components (Gabor patches) to MIPS induced by plaids created by combining pairs of components. If motion signals at the lowest level of motion analysis (V1) influence position assignment, MIPS from plaids should reflect the position shift induced by each component when presented separately. On the other hand, if signals generated in MT (or later) influence perceived position, then MIPS from plaids should be consistent with a motion integration computation on the components. Results showed that MIPS from plaids is larger than the MIPS obtained from individual components, and can be explained by the output of an integration process that combines intersection-of-constraints and vector-sum computations.

[1]  Paul V McGraw,et al.  Motion-Sensitive Neurones in V5/MT Modulate Perceived Spatial Position , 2004, Current Biology.

[2]  Fang Fang,et al.  Strong influence of test patterns on the perception of motion aftereffect and position. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[3]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on perceived position , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion adaptation shifts apparent position without the motion aftereffect , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  J. Zanker,et al.  The movement of motion-defined contours can bias perceived position , 2009, Biology Letters.

[6]  Allan Pantle,et al.  An energy model of interframe interval effects in single-step apparent motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[7]  David Whitney,et al.  Position shifts following crowded second-order motion adaptation reveal processing of local and global motion without awareness. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[8]  Jeounghoon Kim,et al.  Perceived motion in the vector sum direction , 1994, Vision Research.

[9]  David Whitaker,et al.  Motion Adaptation Distorts Perceived Visual Position , 2002, Current Biology.

[10]  David Whitney,et al.  Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Perceived direction of moving two-dimensional patterns depends on duration, contrast and eccentricity , 1992, Vision Research.

[12]  Allan Pantle,et al.  Visual resolution of motion ambiguity with periodic luminance- and contrast-domain stimuli , 1992, Vision Research.

[13]  L. Bowns Evidence for a Feature Tracking Explanation of Why Type II Plaids Move in the Vector Sum Direction at Short Durations , 1996, Vision Research.

[14]  J H van Hateren,et al.  Simulating human cones from mid-mesopic up to high-photopic luminances. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[15]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  Spatial and temporal properties of the illusory motion-induced position shift for drifting stimuli , 2007, Vision Research.

[16]  Szonya Durant,et al.  Temporal dependence of local motion induced shifts in perceived position , 2004, Vision Research.

[17]  E. Adelson,et al.  Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.

[18]  David Whitaker,et al.  Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  Johannes M. Zanker,et al.  Perceptual deformation induced by visual motion , 2001, Naturwissenschaften.

[20]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. , 1991, Vision research.

[21]  J. Gaddum Probit Analysis , 1948, Nature.

[22]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Illusory Displacement of Equiluminous Kinetic Edges , 1990, Perception.

[23]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Motion distorts perceived depth , 2003, Vision Research.

[24]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[25]  Nicolaas Prins,et al.  On the perceived location of global motion , 2002, Vision Research.

[26]  S. McKee,et al.  Statistical properties of forced-choice psychometric functions: Implications of probit analysis , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Influence of motion signals on the perceived position of spatial pattern , 1999, Nature.

[28]  David Alais,et al.  Large shifts in perceived motion direction reveal multiple global motion solutions , 2006, Vision Research.

[29]  Y. Dan,et al.  Asymmetry in Visual Cortical Circuits Underlying Motion-Induced Perceptual Mislocalization , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  George Mather,et al.  Distinct position assignment mechanisms revealed by cross-order motion , 2008, Vision Research.

[31]  David Whitaker,et al.  Non-veridical size perception of expanding and contracting objects , 1999, Vision Research.

[32]  R. Snowden,et al.  Shifts in perceived position following adaptation to visual motion , 1998, Current Biology.