Accommodation, occlusion, and disparity matching are used to guide reaching: a comparison of actual versus virtual environments.

The authors used a virtual environment to investigate visual control of reaching and monocular and binocular perception of egocentric distance, size, and shape. With binocular vision, the results suggested use of disparity matching. This was tested and confirmed in the virtual environment by eliminating other information about contact of hand and target. Elimination of occlusion of hand by target destabilized monocular but not binocular performance. Because the virtual environment entails accommodation of an image beyond reach, the authors predicted overestimation of egocentric distances in the virtual relative to actual environment. This was confirmed. The authors used -2 diopter glasses to reduce the focal distance in the virtual environment. Overestimates were reduced by half. The authors conclude that calibration of perception is required for accurate feedforward reaching and that disparity matching is optimal visual information for calibration.

[1]  H. Leibowitz,et al.  Role of changes in accommodation and convergence in the perception of size. , 1966, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[2]  J E Cutting,et al.  Comparing effects of the horizontal-vertical illusion on grip scaling and judgment: relative versus absolute, not perception versus action. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  J S Tittle,et al.  Recovery of 3-D shape from binocular disparity and structure from motion , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  K J Ciuffreda,et al.  Accommodation and Apparent Distance , 1988, Perception.

[5]  S. Ullman The Interpretation of Visual Motion , 1979 .

[6]  M. Mon-Williams,et al.  The use of vergence information in the programming of prehension , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  F. Pavani,et al.  Are perception and action affected differently by the Titchener circles illusion? , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  E. Sture Eriksson Movement parallax during locomotion , 1974 .

[9]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Optical Velocity Patterns, Velocity-Sensitive Neurons, and Space Perception: A Hypothesis , 1974, Perception.

[10]  J T Todd,et al.  The perception of 3-dimensional affine structure from minimal apparent motion sequences , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  Nathaniel I. Durlach,et al.  Telepresence, time delay and adaptation , 1991 .

[12]  J R Tresilian,et al.  Ordinal depth information from accommodation? , 2000, Ergonomics.

[13]  Daniel S. Mcconnell,et al.  Reaching measures of monocular distance perception: Forward versus side-to-side head movements and haptic feedback , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  M Gentilucci,et al.  Dissociation between perception and visuomotor transformation during reproduction of remembered distances. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Christopher C. Pagano,et al.  Comparing measures of monocular distance perception: Verbal and reaching errors are not correlated. , 1998 .

[16]  O. Faugeras Three-dimensional computer vision: a geometric viewpoint , 1993 .

[17]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  The Effect of Pictorial Illusion on Prehension and Perception , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  W. M. Smith,et al.  The effects of delayed and displaced visual feedback on motor control. , 1980, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  R. B. Post,et al.  Is There Dissociation of Perceptual and Motor Responses to Figural Illusions? , 1996, Perception.

[20]  R. Welch Perceptual Modification: Adapting to Altered Sensory Environments , 1978 .

[21]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  Optical flow from eye movement with head immobilized: “Ocular occlusion” beyond the nose , 1993, Vision Research.

[22]  Mark Mon-Williams,et al.  Natural problems for stereoscopic depth perception in virtual environments , 1995, Vision Research.

[23]  M. Braunstein Depth perception in rotating dot patterns: effects of numerosity and perspective. , 1962, Journal of experimental psychology.

[24]  J S Tittle,et al.  The visual perception of three-dimensional length. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  B J Rogers,et al.  The Appearance of Surfaces Specified by Motion Parallax and Binocular Disparity , 1989, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[26]  M. Jeannerod The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[27]  G. Johansson,et al.  Monocular Movement Parallax and Near-Space Perception , 1973 .

[28]  H. Ono,et al.  Monocular stereopsis with and without head movement. , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[30]  D. A. Owens,et al.  Accommodation, convergence, and distance perception in low illumination. , 1980, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.

[31]  J S Tittle,et al.  The perception of shape and curvedness from binocular stereopsis and structure from motion , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  A. S. Gilinsky Perceived size and distance in visual space. , 1951, Psychological review.

[33]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992 .

[34]  M. Goodale,et al.  Does a monocularly presented size-contrast illusion influence grip aperture? , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  Walter C. Gogel,et al.  A comparison of oculomotor and motion parallax cues of egocentric distance , 1979, Vision Research.

[36]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.

[37]  Harold Alfred Swenson,et al.  The Relative Influence of Accommodation and Convergence in the Judgment of Distance , 1932 .

[38]  J. Elashoff,et al.  Multiple Regression in Behavioral Research. , 1975 .

[39]  Jan P. Allebach,et al.  Human vision, visual processing, and digital display II; Proceedings of the Meeting, San Jose, CA, Feb. 27-Mar. 1, 1991 , 1991 .

[40]  G P Bingham,et al.  The necessity of a perception-action approach to definite distance perception: monocular distance perception to guide reaching. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  Monocular Egocentric Distance Information Generated by Head Movement , 1994 .

[42]  J T Todd,et al.  Distortions of Three-Dimensional Space in the Perceptual Analysis of Motion and Stereo , 1995, Perception.

[43]  R. Gregory The intelligent eye , 1970 .

[44]  John M. Foley,et al.  Visually directed pointing as a function of target distance, direction, and available cues , 1972 .

[45]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  Perceiving the size of trees: form as information about scale , 1993 .

[46]  J T Todd,et al.  Perception of local three-dimensional shape. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  S. Ellis Pictorial communication in virtual and real environments , 1991 .

[48]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[49]  Loong Fah Cheong,et al.  Visual space distortion , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[50]  E. Brenner,et al.  Judging distance from ocular convergence , 1998, Vision Research.

[51]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Binocular Vision and Stereopsis , 1996 .

[52]  Green Bf Figure coherence in the kinetic depth effect. , 1961 .

[53]  M. Fahle,et al.  Grasping Visual Illusions: No Evidence for a Dissociation Between Perception and Action , 2000, Psychological science.

[54]  Robert T. Hennessy,et al.  Oculomotor adjustments and size constancy , 1972 .

[55]  G P Bingham,et al.  Distortions in definite distance and shape perception as measured by reaching without and with haptic feedback. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  R. Held,et al.  Adaptation to displaced and delayed visual feedback from the hand. , 1966 .

[57]  Woodrow Barfield,et al.  Virtual environments and advanced interface design , 1995 .

[58]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Hand and brain : the neurophysiology and psychology of hand movements , 1996 .

[59]  R. Toye,et al.  The effect of viewing position on the perceived layout of space , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[60]  Anne E. Garing,et al.  Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  Chris Shaw,et al.  On temporal-spatial realism in the virtual reality environment , 1991, UIST '91.

[62]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[63]  M F Bradshaw,et al.  The Effect of Pre-Movement Delays on Pointing Accuracy in Middle Childhood , 1998, Perception.

[64]  B J Rogers,et al.  Visual and nonvisual information disambiguate surfaces specified by motion parallax , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[65]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  The rate of adaptation to displacement prisms remains constant despite acquisition of rapid calibration , 1999 .

[66]  H. Lee Task,et al.  Visually coupled systems hardware and the human interface , 1995 .

[67]  M. Mon-Williams,et al.  Some Recent Studies on the Extraretinal Contribution to Distance Perception , 1999, Perception.

[68]  J T Todd,et al.  The perceptual analysis of structure from motion for rotating objects undergoing affine stretching transformations , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[69]  B Rogers,et al.  Motion Parallax as an Independent Cue for Depth Perception , 1979, Perception.

[70]  P. Viviani,et al.  Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[71]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  The Visual Pathways Mediating Perception and Prehension , 1996 .

[72]  E. Johnston Systematic distortions of shape from stereopsis , 1991, Vision Research.

[73]  J. L. Smith,et al.  5 Programming of Stereotyped Limb Movements by Spinal Generators , 1980 .

[74]  M. Wagner,et al.  The metric of visual space , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[75]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[76]  J F Norman,et al.  The visual perception of rigid motion from constant flow fields , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[77]  James R. Tresilian,et al.  Increasing confidence in vergence as a cue to distance , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[78]  Pantazis Mouroulis Visual Instrumentation: Optical Design and Engineering Principles , 1999 .

[79]  M J Steinbach,et al.  Motion parallax judgements of depth as a function of the direction and type of head movement. , 1991, Canadian journal of psychology.

[80]  James T. Todd,et al.  Systematic distortion of perceived three-dimensional structure from motion and binocular stereopsis. , 1995 .

[81]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[82]  John C. Baird,et al.  Quantitative functions for size and distance judgments , 1967 .