Role of childhood aerobic fitness in successful street crossing.

UNLABELLED Increased aerobic fitness is associated with improved cognition, brain health, and academic achievement during preadolescence. PURPOSE In this study, we extended these findings by examining the relationship between aerobic fitness and an everyday real-world task: street crossing. Because street crossing can be a dangerous multitask challenge and is a leading cause of injury in children, it is important to find ways to improve pedestrian safety. METHODS A street intersection was modeled in a virtual environment, and higher-fit (n = 13, 7 boys) and lower-fit (n = 13, 5 boys) 8- to 10-yr-old children, as determined by V˙O(2max) testing, navigated trafficked roads by walking on a treadmill that was integrated with an immersive virtual world. Child pedestrians crossed the street while undistracted, listening to music, or conversing on a hands-free cellular phone. RESULTS Cell phones impaired street crossing success rates compared with the undistracted or music conditions for all participants (P = 0.004), a result that supports previous research. However, individual differences in aerobic fitness influenced these patterns (fitness × condition interaction, P = 0.003). Higher-fit children maintained street crossing success rates across all three conditions (paired t-tests, all P > 0.4), whereas lower-fit children showed decreased success rates when on the phone, relative to the undistracted (P = 0.018) and music (P = 0.019) conditions. CONCLUSIONS The results suggest that higher levels of childhood aerobic fitness may attenuate the impairment typically associated with multitasking during street crossing. It is possible that superior cognitive abilities of higher-fit children play a role in the performance differences during complex real-world tasks.

[1]  Michael A. Regan,et al.  Division of Attention: The Single-Channel Hypothesis Revisited , 1989 .

[2]  James M. Tanner,et al.  Growth at Adolescence , 1956 .

[3]  Russell A Poldrack,et al.  Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  D. Ludwig Childhood obesity--the shape of things to come. , 2007, The New England journal of medicine.

[5]  Johnny Dyreborg,et al.  Protocol for a mixed-methods study on leader-based interventions in construction contractors' safety commitments , 2010, Injury Prevention.

[6]  A. Kramer,et al.  Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[7]  C. L. M. The Psychology of Attention , 1890, Nature.

[8]  Joan Severson,et al.  Validation of virtual reality as a tool to understand and prevent child pedestrian injury. , 2008, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[9]  David M Murray,et al.  Survey development for assessing correlates of young adolescents' eating. , 2002, American journal of health behavior.

[10]  D. Navon Resources—a theoretical soup stone? , 1984 .

[11]  E. Shvartz,et al.  Aerobic fitness norms for males and females aged 6 to 75 years: a review. , 1990, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[12]  John G. Gaspar,et al.  Walking and talking: dual-task effects on street crossing behavior in older adults. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[13]  Katherine W. Byington,et al.  Effect of Cell Phone Distraction on Pediatric Pedestrian Injury Risk , 2009, Pediatrics.

[14]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  A neuroimaging investigation of the association between aerobic fitness, hippocampal volume, and memory performance in preadolescent children , 2010, Brain Research.

[15]  C. Hillman,et al.  Physical fitness and academic achievement in third- and fifth-grade students. , 2007, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[16]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  The Structure of Attentional Resources , 1980 .

[17]  Arthur F Kramer,et al.  Pedestrians, vehicles, and cell phones. , 2010, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[18]  Frank Drews,et al.  Profiles in Driver Distraction: Effects of Cell Phone Conversations on Younger and Older Drivers , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[19]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  Basal Ganglia Volume Is Associated with Aerobic Fitness in Preadolescent Children , 2010, Developmental Neuroscience.

[20]  D. Amso,et al.  Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  D. Strayer,et al.  Supertaskers: Profiles in extraordinary multitasking ability , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  Christopher R. Johnson,et al.  Cardiorespiratory Fitness and the Flexible Modulation of Cognitive Control in Preadolescent Children , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Melissa L. Allen,et al.  Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test , 2021, Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders.

[24]  Matthew B. Pontifex,et al.  Aerobic fitness and cognitive development: Event-related brain potential and task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[25]  John Duncan,et al.  The demonstration of capacity limitation , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  Kristen E Beede,et al.  Engrossed in conversation: the impact of cell phones on simulated driving performance. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[27]  David C Schwebel,et al.  Using virtual reality to train children in safe street-crossing skills , 2010, Injury Prevention.

[28]  P. Thompson,et al.  ACSM's Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription , 1995 .

[29]  L. Hildman,et al.  Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test , 1993 .