The time course and specificity of perceptual deterioration.

Repeated within-day testing on a texture discrimination task leads to retinotopically specific decreases in performance. Although perceptual learning has been shown to be highly specific to the retinotopic location and characteristics of the trained stimulus, the specificity of perceptual deterioration has not been studied. We investigated the similarities between learning and deterioration by examining whether deterioration transfers to new distractor or target orientations or to the untrained eye. Participants performed a texture discrimination task in three one-hour sessions. We tested the specificity of deterioration in the final session by switching either the orientation of the background or the target elements by 90 degrees. We found that performance deteriorated steadily both within and across the first two sessions and was specific to the target but not the distractor orientation. In a separate experiment, we found that deterioration transferred to the untrained eye. Changes in performance were independent of reported sleepiness and awareness of stimulus changes, arguing against the possibility that perceptual deterioration is due to general fatigue. Rather, we hypothesize that perceptual deterioration may be caused by changes in the ability for attention to selectively enhance the responses of relatively low-level orientation-selective sensory neurons, possibly within the primary visual cortex. Further, the differences in specificity profiles between learning and deterioration suggest separate underlying mechanisms that occur within the same cortical area.

[1]  Alain Muzet,et al.  Influence of age, speed and duration of monotonous driving task in traffic on the driver’s useful visual field , 2004, Vision Research.

[2]  D. Moore,et al.  Early and rapid perceptual learning , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Bruce G. Cumming,et al.  Orientation tuning for disparity defined edges in Macaque V2 , 2004 .

[4]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Perceptual learning and top-down influences in primary visual cortex , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  John G Milton,et al.  On the Road to Automatic: Dynamic Aspects in the Development of Expertise , 2004, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[6]  Cong Yu,et al.  Perceptual learning in contrast discrimination and the (minimal) role of context. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[7]  R. Gray Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: expertise differences, choking, and slumps. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[8]  J M Dambrosia,et al.  Abnormalities of spatial discrimination in focal and generalized dystonia. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Sleep-dependent learning: a nap is as good as a night , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  R. Nudo Retuning the misfiring brain , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  A. Muzet,et al.  Effect of sleep deprivation and driving duration on the useful visual field in younger and older subjects during simulator driving , 2003, Vision Research.

[12]  B. Rockstroh,et al.  Effective behavioral treatment of focal hand dystonia in musicians alters somatosensory cortical organization , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  A. A. Levin,et al.  The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Robert A. Jacobs,et al.  Comparing perceptual learning across tasks: A review , 2002 .

[15]  Misha Tsodyks,et al.  Context-enabled learning in the human visual system , 2002, Nature.

[16]  Manjit,et al.  Neurology , 1912, NeuroImage.

[17]  J. Hobson,et al.  Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  J. Liepert,et al.  Effects of passive tactile co‐activation on median ulnar nerve representation in human S1 , 2000, Neuroreport.

[19]  C. Frith,et al.  Inattentional blindness versus inattentional amnesia for fixated but ignored words. , 1999, Science.

[20]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  D. Heeger,et al.  Spatial attention affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  D. Somers,et al.  Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  B. Rockstroh,et al.  Alteration of digital representations in somatosensory cortex in focal hand dystonia , 1998, Neuroreport.

[24]  Z Liu,et al.  Simultaneous learning of motion discrimination in two directions. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[25]  L. Welch,et al.  Velocity-dependent improvements in single-dot direction discrimination , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[27]  W M Jenkins,et al.  A primate genesis model of focal dystonia and repetitive strain injury , 1996, Neurology.

[28]  G A Orban,et al.  Interocular transfer in perceptual learning of a pop-out discrimination task. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[29]  J. Mollon,et al.  Three remarks on perceptual learning. , 1996, Spatial vision.

[30]  D. Levi,et al.  Perceptual learning in parafoveal vision , 1995, Vision Research.

[31]  G. Orban,et al.  Human perceptual learning in identifying the oblique orientation: retinotopy, orientation specificity and monocularity. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[32]  D. Levi,et al.  Perceptual learning in vernier acuity: What is learned? , 1995, Vision Research.

[33]  A. Karni,et al.  The time course of learning a visual skill , 1993, Nature.

[34]  D Sagi,et al.  Where practice makes perfect in texture discrimination: evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  William M. Jenkins,et al.  Neocortical representational dynamics in adult primates: Implications for neuropsychology , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  R. Barker,et al.  Movement Disorders , 1994, Stereotactic and Functional Neurosurgery.

[37]  A. Fiorentini,et al.  Perceptual learning specific for orientation and spatial frequency , 1980, Nature.

[38]  C. L. Hull Essentials of behavior , 1974 .

[39]  J. F. Mackworth,et al.  Vigilance and attention: A signal detection approach , 1970 .