Quality of evidence revealing subtle gender biases in science is in the eye of the beholder

Significance Ever-growing empirical evidence documents a gender bias against women and their research—and favoring men—in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) fields. Our research examined how receptive the scientific and public communities are to experimental evidence demonstrating this gender bias, which may contribute to women’s underrepresentation within STEM. Results from our three experiments, using general-public and university faculty samples, demonstrated that men evaluate the quality of research unveiling this bias as less meritorious than do women. These findings may inform and fuel self-correction efforts within STEM to reduce gender bias, bolster objectivity and diversity in STEM workforces, and enhance discovery, education, and achievement. Scientists are trained to evaluate and interpret evidence without bias or subjectivity. Thus, growing evidence revealing a gender bias against women—or favoring men—within science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) settings is provocative and raises questions about the extent to which gender bias may contribute to women’s underrepresentation within STEM fields. To the extent that research illustrating gender bias in STEM is viewed as convincing, the culture of science can begin to address the bias. However, are men and women equally receptive to this type of experimental evidence? This question was tested with three randomized, double-blind experiments—two involving samples from the general public (n = 205 and 303, respectively) and one involving a sample of university STEM and non-STEM faculty (n = 205). In all experiments, participants read an actual journal abstract reporting gender bias in a STEM context (or an altered abstract reporting no gender bias in experiment 3) and evaluated the overall quality of the research. Results across experiments showed that men evaluate the gender-bias research less favorably than women, and, of concern, this gender difference was especially prominent among STEM faculty (experiment 2). These results suggest a relative reluctance among men, especially faculty men within STEM, to accept evidence of gender biases in STEM. This finding is problematic because broadening the participation of underrepresented people in STEM, including women, necessarily requires a widespread willingness (particularly by those in the majority) to acknowledge that bias exists before transformation is possible.

[1]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[2]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[3]  Amy M. Hightower,et al.  Science and Engineering Indicators , 1993 .

[4]  L. Ross,et al.  Biased Assimilation and Attitude Polarization: The Effects of Prior Theories on Subsequently Considered Evidence , 1979 .

[5]  Anne E. Lincoln,et al.  Gender Segregation in Elite Academic Science , 2012 .

[6]  W. Clinton State of the Union Address , 2003 .

[7]  Jason M. Sheltzer,et al.  Elite male faculty in the life sciences employ fewer women , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  Virginia Valian,et al.  Laboratory life: Scientists of the world speak up for equality , 2013, Nature.

[9]  M. Graham,et al.  Scientific Diversity Interventions , 2014, Science.

[10]  Steve Olson,et al.  Engage to Excel: Producing One Million Additional College Graduates with Degrees in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics. Report to the President. , 2012 .

[11]  Michael I Norton,et al.  Whites See Racism as a Zero-Sum Game That They Are Now Losing , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[12]  Henrietta O'Connor,et al.  Gender balance: Women are funded more fairly in social science , 2015, Nature.

[13]  I. de Melo-Martín,et al.  Interpreting Evidence: Why Values Can Matter As Much As Science , 2012, Perspectives in biology and medicine.

[14]  Remove social barriers , 2013 .

[15]  Cassidy R. Sugimoto,et al.  Bibliometrics: Global gender disparities in science , 2013, Nature.

[16]  C. Moss‐Racusin,et al.  Can Evidence Impact Attitudes? Public Reactions to Evidence of Gender Bias in STEM Fields , 2015 .

[17]  M. Mahoney Publication prejudices: An experimental study of confirmatory bias in the peer review system , 1977, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[18]  Ulysses S. Grant,et al.  State of the Union Address , 2003 .

[19]  Wei Huang,et al.  Collaborating with People Like Me: Ethnic Co-Authorship within the Us , 2014, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[20]  S. Ceci,et al.  Understanding current causes of women's underrepresentation in science , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  J. Kasof,et al.  Sex bias in the naming of stimulus persons. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  Thomas D. Snyder,et al.  Digest of Education Statistics , 1994 .

[23]  S. Shields,et al.  The Impact of the Workshop Activity for Gender Equity Simulation in the Academy (WAGES-Academic) in Demonstrating Cumulative Effects of Gender Bias , 2011 .

[24]  C. Stangor,et al.  The role of intent and harm in judgments of prejudice and discrimination. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  A Decade of System Justification Theory: Accumulated Evidence of Conscious and Unconscious Bolstering of the Status Quo , 2004 .

[26]  R. Steinpreis,et al.  The Impact of Gender on the Review of the Curricula Vitae of Job Applicants and Tenure Candidates: A National Empirical Study , 1999 .

[27]  L. Festinger,et al.  A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 2017 .

[28]  M. Brewer The Psychology of Prejudice: Ingroup Love and Outgroup Hate? , 1999 .

[29]  Eric Luis Uhlmann,et al.  "I think it, therefore it's true": Effects of self-perceived objectivity on hiring discrimination. , 2007 .

[30]  Lu Hong,et al.  Groups of diverse problem solvers can outperform groups of high-ability problem solvers. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  A. Griffin,et al.  Fewer invited talks by women in evolutionary biology symposia , 2013, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[32]  Silvia Knobloch-Westerwick,et al.  The Matilda Effect in Science Communication , 2013 .

[33]  H. Tajfel,et al.  The Social Identity Theory of Intergroup Behavior. , 2004 .

[34]  M. Banaji,et al.  Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.

[35]  Helen Shen Inequality quantified: Mind the gender gap , 2013, Nature.

[36]  P. Mcintosh White privilege: Unpacking the invisible knapsack. , 2003 .

[37]  Ernesto Reuben,et al.  How stereotypes impair women’s careers in science , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Leila T. Worth,et al.  Processing of persuasive in-group messages. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[39]  K. W. Phillips,et al.  Rethinking the Baseline in Diversity Research , 2014, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[40]  Heather M. Claypool,et al.  Identity salience moderates processing of group-relevant information , 2010 .

[41]  M. Graham,et al.  Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  P. Devine Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. , 1989 .

[43]  Bertjan Doosje,et al.  Self and social identity. , 2002, Annual review of psychology.

[44]  Francis S Collins,et al.  Weaving a Richer Tapestry in Biomedical Science , 2011, Science.

[45]  C. Wennerås,et al.  Nepotism and sexism in peer-review , 1997, Nature.

[46]  Eve Fine,et al.  Promoting Institutional Change Through Bias Literacy. , 2012, Journal of diversity in higher education.

[47]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Neural correlates of automatic beliefs about gender and race , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[48]  Scott E. Page,et al.  Making the Difference: Applying a Logic of Diversity , 2007 .

[49]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[50]  Shulamit Kahn,et al.  Does Science Promote Women? Evidence from Academia 1973-2001 , 2006 .

[51]  S. Shields,et al.  How to Talk about Gender Inequity in the Workplace: Using WAGES as an Experiential Learning Tool to Reduce Reactance and Promote Self-Efficacy , 2012 .

[52]  Nyla R. Branscombe,et al.  The good, the bad, and the manly: Threats to one's prototypicality and evaluations of fellow in-group members. , 2001 .