Rapid acquisition of phonological alternations by infants

We explore whether infants can learn novel phonological alternations on the basis of distributional information. In Experiment 1, two groups of 12-month-old infants were familiarized with artificial languages whose distributional properties exhibited either stop or fricative voicing alternations. At test, infants in the two exposure groups had different preferences for novel sequences involving voiced and voiceless stops and fricatives, suggesting that each group had internalized a different familiarization alternation. In Experiment 2, 8.5-month-olds exhibited the same patterns of preference. In Experiments 3 and 4, we investigated whether infants' preferences were driven solely by preferences for sequences of high transitional probability. Although 8.5-month-olds in Experiment 3 were sensitive to the relative probabilities of sequences in the familiarization stimuli, only 12-month-olds in Experiment 4 showed evidence of having grouped alternating segments into a single functional category. Taken together, these results suggest a developmental trajectory for the acquisition of phonological alternations using distributional cues in the input.

[1]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .

[2]  Jessica Maye,et al.  Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination , 2002, Cognition.

[3]  William E. Cooper,et al.  Syntax and Speech , 1980 .

[4]  J. Werker,et al.  Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life , 1984 .

[5]  H. Bortfeld Running Head: EMPHATIC STRESS Early Word Recognition may be Stress-full , 2007 .

[6]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. , 1994 .

[7]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Acquisition of English Number Marking: The Singular-Plural Distinction , 2006 .

[8]  Katherine S White,et al.  Preference and processing: The role of speech affect in early spoken word recognition , 2004 .

[9]  John Kingston,et al.  Papers in Laboratory Phonology: Index of names , 1990 .

[10]  B. MacWhinney The Acquisition Of Morphophonology , 1978 .

[11]  Pienie Zwitserlood,et al.  Variation and assimilation in German: Consequences for lexical access and representation , 2001 .

[12]  Katherine S. White,et al.  A Statistical Basis for Speech Sound Discrimination , 2003, Language and speech.

[13]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[14]  Kristine H. Onishi,et al.  Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience , 2003, Cognition.

[15]  Colin Wilson,et al.  Learning Phonology With Substantive Bias: An Experimental and Computational Study of Velar Palatalization , 2006, Cogn. Sci..

[16]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Advances in infancy research , 1981 .

[17]  P. Kuhl Perception of auditory equivalence classes for speech in early infancy , 1983 .

[18]  LouAnn Gerken,et al.  Learning Phonemes: How Far Can the Input Take Us? , 2001 .

[19]  D. Slobin,et al.  Studies of child language development , 1973 .

[20]  P. Eimas Segmental and syllabic representations in the perception of speech by young infants. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  The role of talker-specific information in word segmentation by infants. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  Caroline Féry,et al.  Variation and Gradience in Phonetics and Phonology , 2009 .

[23]  Katherine S White,et al.  Receptive Grammatical Knowledge of Familiar Content Words and Inflection in 16-Month-Olds. , 2007, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[24]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  When cues collide: use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[25]  Katherine S. White,et al.  Building a Word-Form Lexicon in the Face of Variable Input: Influences of Pitch and Amplitude on Early Spoken Word Recognition , 2008 .

[26]  Eugene Buckley,et al.  On the Learning of Arbitrary Phonological Rules , 2005 .

[27]  Colin Phillips,et al.  The influence of meaning on the perception of speech sounds. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Visual statistical learning in infancy , 2010 .

[29]  K. Stevens,et al.  Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. , 1992, Science.

[30]  Peter W. Jusczyk,et al.  Representation of Speech Sounds by Young Infants. , 1987 .

[31]  D. Slobin Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar , 1973 .

[32]  D. Whalen,et al.  Lexical effects in the perception and production of American English /p/ allophones , 1997 .

[33]  Elisabeth Selkirk,et al.  Phonology and Syntax: The Relation between Sound and Structure , 1984 .

[34]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants’ sensitivity to allophonic cues for word segmentation , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[35]  Paul D. Allopenna,et al.  Phonological and acoustic bases for earliest grammatical category assignment: a cross-linguistic perspective , 1998, Journal of Child Language.

[36]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Mechanisms of phonological inference in speech perception. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  Jacques Mehler,et al.  How do 4-day-old infants categorize multisyllabic utterances? , 1993 .

[38]  D. Scott,et al.  Segmental Phonology and the Perception of Syntactic Structure. , 1984 .

[39]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Mechanisms of phonological inference in speech perception. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Word Segmentation by 8-Month-Olds: When Speech Cues Count More Than Statistics , 2001 .

[41]  J. Werker,et al.  Adult and infant perception of two English phones. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[42]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  The mental representation of lexical form: A phonological approach to the recognition lexicon , 1991, Cognition.

[43]  J. Morgan,et al.  Mommy and Me , 2005, Psychological science.

[44]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Phonological variation and inference in lexical access. , 1996 .

[45]  E. W. Ames,et al.  A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. , 1988 .

[46]  Alan S. Prince,et al.  Prosodic Morphology 1986 , 1996 .

[47]  R. Gómez Variability and Detection of Invariant Structure , 2002, Psychological science.

[48]  Jessica Maye,et al.  Statistical Cues Facilitate Infants ’ Discrimination of Difficult Phonetic Contrasts , 2002 .

[49]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  Pattern induction by infant language learners. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[50]  B. MacWhinney Miniature linguistic systems as tests of the use of universal operating principles in second-language learning by children and adults , 1983 .

[51]  D. Slobin Crosslinguistic Evidence for the Language-making Capacity , 1985 .

[52]  D. Gow Feature parsing: Feature cue mapping in spoken word recognition , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[53]  C. Baltaxe,et al.  Principles of phonology , 1969 .

[54]  Mary E. Hughes,et al.  Boston University Conference on Language Development , 1979 .

[55]  Wolfgang Klein,et al.  Second Language Acquisition: The process of language acquisition , 1986 .

[56]  C. Laeufer,et al.  Phonology and Phonetic Evidence: Papers in Laboratory Phonology IV , 1995 .

[57]  D. Slobin The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition , 1987 .

[58]  Laurel Fais,et al.  Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese , 2007, Cognition.

[59]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .

[60]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[61]  B. Schölkopf,et al.  Edinburgh Research Explorer Interpolating between types and tokens by estimating power-law generators , 2006 .

[62]  J. Nadal,et al.  The acquisition of allophonic rules: Statistical learning with linguistic constraints , 2006, Cognition.

[63]  Sharon Peperkamp,et al.  Coping with phonological variation in early lexical acquisition , 2000 .

[64]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants′ Detection of the Sound Patterns of Words in Fluent Speech , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[65]  J. Werker,et al.  Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[66]  John P. Pinto,et al.  Rapid Gains in Speed of Verbal Processing by Infants in the 2nd Year , 1998 .

[67]  Sharon Peperkamp,et al.  Learning the mapping from surface to underlying representations in an artificial language , 2006 .

[68]  Sharon Peperkamp,et al.  The role of phonetic naturalness in phonological rule acquisition , 2006 .

[69]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Do infants segment words or recurring contiguous patterns? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[70]  David Bamman,et al.  Proceedings of the 30th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development , 2006 .

[71]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Phonological variation and inference in lexical access. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[72]  Francis Nolan,et al.  Phonology and Phonetic Evidence: The influence of syntactic structure on [s] to [∫] assimilation , 1995 .

[73]  D. Napoli Elements of tone, stress, and intonation , 1978 .