Mastery of structured quantities like finger or dice patterns predict arithmetic performance

In the present study, we investigated whether structured quantities like finger or dice patterns are enumerated better than unstructured quantities because they may not require counting. Moreover, we hypothesized children’s mastery of structured quantities to predict their later arithmetic performance longitudinally. In particular, we expected that children more proficient in enumerating structured quantities early in their numerical development, should develop more effective calculation strategies later because they may rely on counting less. Therefore, we conducted a longitudinal study (including 116 children, 58 girls) over the course of about 7 months from preschool (at about 6 years of age) to the middle of first grade. Results showed that structured quantities were indeed enumerated more accurately and faster than unstructured quantities in preschool. Additionally, we observed significant associations of enumeration of structured and unstructured with children’s addition performance in first grade. However, regression analysis indicated only enumeration of structured but not unstructured quantities to significantly predict later addition performance. In sum, this longitudinal study clearly indicates that mastery of structured quantities seems to be beneficial for children’s development of basic arithmetic abilities.

[1]  Pierre Barrouillet,et al.  Predicting arithmetical achievement from neuro-psychological performance: a longitudinal study , 1998, Cognition.

[2]  Miriam Lüken,et al.  Young Children’s Structure Sense , 2012 .

[3]  L. Martignon,et al.  Finger gnosis predicts a unique but small part of variance in initial arithmetic performance. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[4]  Mauro Pesenti,et al.  Finger Numeral Representations: More than Just Another Symbolic Code , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[5]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Do First Graders Make Efficient Use of External Number Representations? The Case of the Twenty-Frame , 2014 .

[6]  David C. Geary,et al.  From infancy to adulthood: the development of numerical abilities , 2009, European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry.

[7]  Miriam M. Lüken,et al.  The Influence of Fostering Children's Patterning Abilities on Their Arithmetic Skills in Grade 1 , 2018 .

[8]  Young children's understanding of division: The relationship between division terms in a noncomputational task. , 1998 .

[9]  Guilherme Wood,et al.  A Hand Full of Numbers: A Role for Offloading in Arithmetics Learning? , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[10]  E. L. Kaufman,et al.  The discrimination of visual number. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.

[11]  Katharina Lambert,et al.  Basic numerical competences in large-scale assessment data: Structure and long-term relevance. , 2018, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[12]  J. Sarama,et al.  Early Childhood Mathematics Education Research: Learning Trajectories for Young Children , 2009 .

[13]  G. Mandler,et al.  Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  The role of fingers in the development of counting and arithmetic skills. , 2015, Acta psychologica.

[15]  François Jouen,et al.  Do young children acquire number words through subitizing or counting , 2004 .

[16]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  Aspects of situated cognition in embodied numerosity: the case of finger counting , 2014, Cognitive Processing.

[17]  K. Moeller,et al.  Spatial Arrangement and Set Size Influence the Coding of Non-symbolic Quantities in the Intraparietal Sulcus , 2018, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[18]  Karin Landerl,et al.  Subitizing and counting in typical and atypical development. , 2011, Developmental science.

[19]  F. Rivera-batiz,et al.  Quantitative Literacy and the Likelihood of Employment among Young Adults in the United States , 1992 .

[20]  D. Clements,et al.  Learning and Teaching Early Math: The Learning Trajectories Approach , 2009 .

[21]  J. Hurry,et al.  The relationship between learning mathematics and general cognitive ability in primary school , 2018, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[22]  M. Noël,et al.  Does finger training increase young children's numerical performance? , 2008, Cortex.

[23]  B. Butterworth,et al.  A Head for Figures , 1999, Science.

[24]  R Rothkegel,et al.  Subitizing and its subprocesses , 2000, Psychological research.

[25]  M. Mitchelmore,et al.  Awareness of pattern and structure in early mathematical development , 2009 .

[26]  M. Noël,et al.  Finger gnosia: a predictor of numerical abilities in children? , 2005, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[27]  John Bishop,et al.  Is the Test Score Decline Responsible for the Productivity Growth Decline? Working Paper No. 87-05. , 1987 .

[28]  Korbinian Moeller,et al.  Human Neuroscience Perspective Article Considering Digits in a Current Model of Numerical Development , 2022 .

[29]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for retrieval and decision processes in mental addition. , 1978 .

[30]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  One, two, three, many - subitizing in active touch. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[31]  E. Spelke,et al.  Numerical abstraction by human infants , 1990, Cognition.

[32]  K. Fuson Children's Counting and Concepts of Number , 1987 .

[33]  B. Butterworth The development of arithmetical abilities. , 2005, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[34]  J. Enns,et al.  Life Span Changes in Visual Enumeration: The Number Discrimination Task , 1996 .

[35]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.

[36]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Are preschoolers who spontaneously create patterns better in mathematics? , 2019, The British journal of educational psychology.

[37]  C. Hulme,et al.  Pattern understanding is a predictor of early reading and arithmetic skills , 2019, Early Childhood Research Quarterly.

[38]  J. Hair Multivariate data analysis , 1972 .

[39]  Daniel P. Keating,et al.  Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. , 1983, Child development.

[40]  D. Geary Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: a 5-year longitudinal study. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[41]  Prentice Starkey,et al.  The early development of numerical reasoning , 1992, Cognition.

[42]  Andreas Obersteiner,et al.  Formal and Informal Learning Environments: Using Games to Support Early Numeracy , 2015 .

[43]  K. C. Fuson,et al.  The acquisition of early number word meanings: A conceptual analysis and review , 1983 .

[44]  Frank Domahs,et al.  Embodied numerosity: Implicit hand-based representations influence symbolic number processing across cultures , 2010, Cognition.

[45]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  The Mathematical Brain , 1999 .

[46]  Frank Domahs,et al.  Mind the gap between both hands: Evidence for internal finger-based number representations in children's mental calculation , 2008, Cortex.

[47]  Andy P. Field,et al.  Discovering Statistics Using Ibm Spss Statistics , 2017 .

[48]  A. Henik,et al.  Do subitizing deficits in developmental dyscalculia involve pattern recognition weakness? , 2013, Developmental science.

[49]  G. Wolters,et al.  Quantification of Small Numbers of Dots: Subitizing or Pattern Recognition? , 1987 .

[50]  J. Bisanz,et al.  The Foundations of Numeracy: Subitizing, Finger Gnosia, and Fine Motor Ability , 2007 .

[51]  S. Carey,et al.  The Representations Underlying Infants' Choice of More: Object Files Versus Analog Magnitudes , 2002, Psychological science.

[52]  The Power of Five: The Step before the Power of Ten , 1986 .

[53]  C. Benz,et al.  Enhancing and analyzing kindergarten teachers’ professional knowledge for early mathematics education , 2018, The Journal of Mathematical Behavior.

[54]  K. Wynn Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[55]  Number Grouping as a Function of Complexity , 1953, The Elementary School Journal.

[56]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  The roles of patterning and spatial skills in early mathematics development , 2019, Early Childhood Research Quarterly.

[57]  M. Barnes,et al.  Working memory and mathematics: A review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. , 2010 .

[58]  C. Escera,et al.  An event-related brain potential study of the arithmetic split effect. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[59]  Michael H. Kutner Applied Linear Statistical Models , 1974 .

[60]  Erica L. Zippert,et al.  Preschoolers' broad mathematics experiences with parents during play. , 2019, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[61]  I. Morocz,et al.  Subitizing is sensitive to the arrangement of objects. , 2013, Experimental psychology.

[62]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.

[63]  S. Pixner,et al.  Early Predictors for Basic Numerical and Magnitude Competencies in Preschool Children—Are They the Same or Different regarding Specific Subgroups? , 2017 .

[64]  Ariel Starr,et al.  Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.