Developmental changes in visual responses to social interactions
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. Soto-Faraco,et al. Visual Search for People Among People , 2019, Psychological science.
[2] L. Papeo,et al. The Representation of Two-Body Shapes in the Human Visual Cortex , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[3] L. Papeo,et al. Seeing social events: The visual specialization for dyadic human-human interactions. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] Kami Koldewyn,et al. Dyadic interaction processing in the posterior temporal cortex , 2019, NeuroImage.
[5] S. Tipper,et al. Bound together: Social binding leads to faster processing, spatial distortion, and enhanced memory of interacting partners. , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[6] Kami Koldewyn,et al. Neural responses to visually observed social interactions , 2018, Neuropsychologia.
[7] R. Saxe,et al. Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years , 2018, Nature Communications.
[8] N. Kanwisher,et al. Perceiving social interactions in the posterior superior temporal sulcus , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[9] Mowei Shen,et al. Two Equals One: Two Human Actions During Social Interaction Are Grouped as One Unit in Working Memory , 2017, Psychological science.
[10] S. Quadflieg,et al. The neuroscience of people watching: how the human brain makes sense of other people's encounters , 2017, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[11] Katie L. H. Gray,et al. Social Interaction Contexts Bias the Perceived Expressions of Interactants , 2017, Emotion.
[12] Salvador Soto-Faraco,et al. The Two-Body Inversion Effect , 2017, Psychological science.
[13] Daniel D. Dilks,et al. Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants , 2017, Nature Communications.
[14] C. Schmitz,et al. Deciphering human motion to discriminate social interactions: a developmental neuroimaging study , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[15] B. Rossion,et al. The neural basis of perceiving person interactions , 2015, Cortex.
[16] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. Functional Organization of Social Perception and Cognition in the Superior Temporal Sulcus , 2015, Cerebral cortex.
[17] Frances Crabbe,et al. Body-selective areas in the visual cortex are less active in children than in adults , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[18] Natacha S. Santos,et al. Perceiving nonverbal behavior: Neural correlates of processing movement fluency and contingency in dyadic interactions , 2014, Human brain mapping.
[19] Rainer Goebel,et al. Development from childhood to adulthood increases morphological and functional inter-individual variability in the right superior temporal cortex , 2013, NeuroImage.
[20] Christine Fawcett,et al. Infants use social context to bind actions into a collaborative sequence. , 2013, Developmental science.
[21] C. Schmitz,et al. From Action to Interaction: Exploring the Contribution of Body Motion Cues to Social Understanding in Typical Development and in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2012, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
[22] Mikko Sams,et al. Naturalistic fMRI Mapping Reveals Superior Temporal Sulcus as the Hub for the Distributed Brain Network for Social Perception , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[23] Riitta Hari,et al. Engagement of amygdala in third‐person view of face‐to‐face interaction , 2012, Human brain mapping.
[24] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. An algorithmic method for functionally defining regions of interest in the ventral visual pathway , 2012, NeuroImage.
[25] N. Kanwisher,et al. Thin-slice perception develops slowly. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[26] Annette M. E. Henderson,et al. “Let’s work together”: What do infants understand about collaborative goals? , 2011, Cognition.
[27] Daniel D. Dilks,et al. Differential selectivity for dynamic versus static information in face-selective cortical regions , 2011, NeuroImage.
[28] Cristina Becchio,et al. Communicative Interactions Improve Visual Detection of Biological Motion , 2011, PloS one.
[29] Jean-Luc Anton,et al. Recruitment of Both the Mirror and the Mentalizing Networks When Observing Social Interactions Depicted by Point-Lights: A Neuroimaging Study , 2011, PloS one.
[30] Patrik Vuilleumier,et al. Differential development of selectivity for faces and bodies in the fusiform gyrus. , 2009, Developmental science.
[31] Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli,et al. Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children. , 2009, Child development.
[32] J. Hamlin,et al. Social evaluation by preverbal infants , 2007, Nature.
[33] Beatriz Luna,et al. Visual category-selectivity for faces, places and objects emerges along different developmental trajectories. , 2007, Developmental science.
[34] K. Grill-Spector,et al. Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[35] K. Pelphrey,et al. School-aged children exhibit domain-specific responses to biological motion , 2006, Social neuroscience.
[36] Peter Neri,et al. Meaningful interactions can enhance visual discrimination of human agents , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[37] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. Divide and conquer: A defense of functional localizers , 2006, NeuroImage.
[38] Gregory McCarthy,et al. Taking an “intentional stance” on eye-gaze shifts: A functional neuroimaging study of social perception in children , 2005, NeuroImage.
[39] Thomas F. Nugent,et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[40] E. Darcy Burgund,et al. Comparison of functional activation foci in children and adults using a common stereotactic space , 2003, NeuroImage.
[41] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[42] C. Frith,et al. Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.
[43] A. Etienne,et al. Seeing relationships: The specialization for a two-body shape in human visual perception , 2019 .
[44] Kristin Shutts,et al. Children use nonverbal cues to make inferences about social power. , 2015, Child development.
[45] S. Blakemore,et al. Developmental changes in the structure of the social brain in late childhood and adolescence. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[46] Jean-Luc Anton,et al. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox , 2010 .