Rhythmic movement in Parkinson’s disease: effects of visual feedback and medication state

Previous studies examining discrete movements of Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients have found that in addition to performing movements that were slower than those of control participants, they exhibit specific deficits in movement coordination and in sensorimotor integration required to accurately guide movements. With medication, movement speed was normalized, but the coordinative aspects of movement were not. This led to the hypothesis that dopaminergic medication more readily compensates for intensive aspects of movement (such as speed), than for coordinative aspects (such as coordination of different limb segments) (Schettino et al., Exp Brain Res 168:186–202, 2006). We tested this hypothesis on rhythmic, continuous movements of the forearm. In our task, target peak speed and amplitude, availability of visual feedback, and medication state (on/off) were varied. We found, consistent with the discrete-movement results, that peak speed (intensive aspect) was normalized by medication, while accuracy, which required coordination of speed and amplitude modulation (coordinative aspect), was not normalized by dopaminergic treatment. However, our findings that amplitude, an intensive aspect of movement, was also not normalized by medication, suggests that a simple pathway gain increase does not act to remediate all intensive aspects of movement to the same extent. While it normalized movement peak speed, it did not normalize movement amplitude. Furthermore, we found that when visual feedback was not available, all participants (PD and controls) made faster movements. The effects of dopaminergic medication and availability of visual feedback on movement speed were additive. The finding that movement speed uniformly increased both in the PD and the control groups suggests that visual feedback may be necessary for calibration of peak speed, otherwise underestimated by the motor control system.

[1]  F. Horak,et al.  Abnormal proprioceptive-motor integration contributes to hypometric postural responses of subjects with parkinson’s disease , 2006, Neuroscience.

[2]  Hermano Igo Krebs,et al.  Parkinson's disease: a motor control study using a wrist robot , 2007, Adv. Robotics.

[3]  Hong Yu,et al.  Role of hyperactive cerebellum and motor cortex in Parkinson's disease , 2007, NeuroImage.

[4]  G. Wooten,et al.  Preliminary FMRI evidence of visual system dysfunction in Parkinson's disease patients with visual hallucinations. , 2006, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[5]  T. Flash,et al.  Kinematic analysis of upper limb trajectories in Parkinson's disease , 1992, Experimental Neurology.

[6]  A. Korczyn,et al.  VISUAL CONTROL OF ARM MOVEMENT IN PARKINSON'S DISEASE , 1996 .

[7]  W. Warren,et al.  Resonance Tuning in Rhythmic Arm Movements. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[8]  K. Flowers,et al.  Movement variability and bradykinesia in Parkinson's disease. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  M. Verrier,et al.  Defective Utilization of Sensory Input as the Basis for Bradykinesia, Rigidity and Decreased Movement Repertoire in Parkinson’s Disease: A Hypothesis , 1984, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[10]  Joseph A. Doeringer,et al.  Intermittency in preplanned elbow movements persists in the absence of visual feedback. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Basal ganglia motor control. I. Nonexclusive relation of pallidal discharge to five movement modes. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  C D Marsden,et al.  Training improves the speed of aimed movements in Parkinson's disease. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  J. Frank,et al.  An evaluation of sensorimotor integration during locomotion toward a target in Parkinson’s disease , 2005, Neuroscience.

[14]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Chaotic Frequency Scaling in a Coupled Oscillator Model for Free Rhythmic Actions , 2008, Neural Computation.

[15]  M. Borutta,et al.  A defect of kinesthesia in Parkinson's disease , 1995, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[16]  J C Rothwell,et al.  Scaling of the size of the first agonist EMG burst during rapid wrist movements in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1986, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[17]  L Proteau,et al.  Aging and motor control. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[18]  Becky G. Farley,et al.  Training BIG to move faster: the application of the speed–amplitude relation as a rehabilitation strategy for people with Parkinson’s disease , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  A. Karniel,et al.  Early switching between movement types: Indication of predictive control? , 2011, Brain Research Bulletin.

[20]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  The timing of arm-trunk coordination is deficient and vision-dependent in Parkinson's patients during reaching movements , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Dopamine replacement therapy does not restore the ability of Parkinsonian patients to make rapid adjustments in motor strategies according to changing sensorimotor contexts. , 2007, Parkinsonism & related disorders.

[22]  J. Mattingley,et al.  An evaluation of the role of internal cues in the pathogenesis of parkinsonian hypokinesia. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  L. Henderson,et al.  The internal control of action and Parkinson's disease: a kinematic analysis of visually-guided and memory-guided prehension movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Kenneth W. Flowers,et al.  Lack of prediction in the motor behaviour of Parkinsonism. , 1978, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  M. Hallett,et al.  A mismatch between kinesthetic and visual perception in Parkinson's disease , 1997, Annals of neurology.

[26]  A. Berardelli,et al.  Sensorimotor integration in movement disorders , 2003, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[27]  Luciano M. Melo,et al.  Abnormal visual activation in Parkinson's disease patients , 2010, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[28]  M. Hallett,et al.  A physiological mechanism of bradykinesia. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  J. Sage,et al.  The interaction of visual and proprioceptive inputs in pointing to actual and remembered targets in Parkinson’s disease , 2001, Neuroscience.

[30]  N Accornero,et al.  Sequential arm movements in patients with Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease and dystonia. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  F. Fazio,et al.  Visual feedback has differential effects on reaching movements in Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease , 2000, Brain Research.

[32]  M. Nomoto,et al.  Levodopa in pregnancy. , 1997, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[33]  C. Markham,et al.  Deficits in orofacial sensorimotor function in Parkinson's disease , 1986, Annals of neurology.

[34]  S. Schaal,et al.  Rhythmic arm movement is not discrete , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  Maria Fatima Silva,et al.  Independent patterns of damage within magno-, parvo- and koniocellular pathways in Parkinson's disease. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Disruptive influences of a cued voluntary shift on coordinated movement in Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  K. Flowers Visual "closed-loop" and "open-loop" characteristics of voluntary movement in patients with Parkinsonism and intention tremor. , 1976, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  D. Eckstein,et al.  Parkinson's disease and dopaminergic therapy—differential effects on movement, reward and cognition , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  N. Hogan,et al.  On rhythmic and discrete movements: reflections, definitions and implications for motor control , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  J. Noth,et al.  Precision grip and Parkinson's disease. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  M. Aminoff,et al.  Sensory perception in Parkinson disease. , 1997, Archives of neurology.

[42]  A. Bastian,et al.  Interaction of levodopa and cues on voluntary reaching in Parkinson's disease , 2002, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[43]  Hermano Igo Krebs,et al.  Non-monotonicity on a spatio-temporally defined cyclic task: evidence of two movement types? , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  J. Summers,et al.  Stride length regulation in Parkinson's disease. Normalization strategies and underlying mechanisms. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  David Abernethy,et al.  Self-Initiated Versus Externally Cued Reaction Times in Parkinson's Disease , 2002, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[46]  D M Corcos,et al.  Control of movement distance in Parkinson's disease , 2001, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[47]  Luis F. Schettino,et al.  Hand preshaping in Parkinson’s disease: effects of visual feedback and medication state , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.