Response Suppression by Automatic Retrieval of Stimulus–Stop Association: Evidence from Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Behavioral studies show that subjects respond more slowly to stimuli to which they previously stopped. This response slowing could be explained by “automatic inhibition” (i.e., the reinstantiation of motor suppression when a stimulus retrieves a stop association). Here we tested this using TMS. In Experiment 1, participants were trained to go or no-go to stimuli. Then, in a test phase, we compared the corticospinal excitability for go stimuli that were previously associated with stopping (no-go_then_go) with go stimuli that were previously associated with going (go_then_go). Corticospinal excitability was reduced for no-go_then_go compared with go_then_go stimuli at a mere 100 msec poststimulus. Although these results fit with automatic inhibition, there was, surprisingly, no suppression for no-go_then_no-go stimuli, although this should occur. We speculated that automatic inhibition lies within a continuum between effortful top–down response inhibition and no inhibition at all. When the need for executive control and active response suppression disappears, so does the manifestation of automatic inhibition. Therefore, it should emerge during go/no-go learning and disappear as performance asymptotes. Consistent with this idea, in Experiment 2, we demonstrated reduced corticospinal excitability for no-go versus go trials most prominently in the midphase of training but it wears off as performance asymptotes. We thus provide neurophysiological evidence for an inhibition mechanism that is automatically reinstantiated when a stimulus retrieves a learned stopping episode, but only in an executive context in which active suppression is required. This demonstrates that automatic and top–down inhibition jointly contribute to goal-directed behavior.

[1]  Carsten Nicolas Boehler,et al.  Pinning down response inhibition in the brain — Conjunction analyses of the Stop-signal task , 2010, NeuroImage.

[2]  Diane Swick,et al.  Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks , 2011, NeuroImage.

[3]  P. Rossini,et al.  Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[4]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Having a goal to stop action is associated with advance control of specific motor representations , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  G. Logan,et al.  Automaticity of cognitive control: goal priming in response-inhibition paradigms. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  Noritaka Kawashima,et al.  Human cortical activities during Go/NoGo tasks with opposite motor control paradigms , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Catherine M. Arrington,et al.  Episodic and semantic components of the compound-stimulus strategy in the explicit task-cuing procedure , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[8]  Koji Jimura,et al.  Activation of Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus during Response Inhibition across Response Modalities , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  Borís Burle,et al.  Mechanisms and Dynamics of Cortical Motor Inhibition in the Stop-signal Paradigm: A TMS Study , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  John Duncan,et al.  The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control , 2010, NeuroImage.

[11]  M. Bellgrove,et al.  Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[12]  G. Logan,et al.  Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Dissociating consciousness from inhibitory control: evidence for unconsciously triggered response inhibition in the stop-signal task. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  F. Parmentier The cognitive determinants of behavioral distraction by deviant auditory stimuli: a review , 2013, Psychological Research.

[15]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Triangulating a Cognitive Control Network Using Diffusion-Weighted Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Functional MRI , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .

[17]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control , 2013, Progress in Neurobiology.

[18]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Inhibition-related Activation in the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus in the Absence of Inhibitory Cues , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Harm Veling,et al.  Putting behavior on hold decreases reward value of need-instrumental objects outside of awareness , 2009 .

[20]  W. Fias,et al.  Repetition priming in the stop signal task: The electrophysiology of sequential effects of stopping , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[22]  Katrijn Houben,et al.  Overcoming the urge to splurge: influencing eating behavior by manipulating inhibitory control. , 2011, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[23]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[24]  A. Wagner,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Cognitive Control and Right Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex: Reflexive Reorienting, Motor Inhibition, and Action Updating , 2022 .

[25]  Katrijn Houben,et al.  Resisting temptation: decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition. , 2011, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[26]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Responding with Restraint: What Are the Neurocognitive Mechanisms? , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  R. Wiers,et al.  Retraining automatic action-tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. , 2010, Addiction.

[28]  Janette L. Smith,et al.  Conflict and inhibition in the cued-Go/NoGo task , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[29]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A model of the go/no-go task. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[30]  Lucas Spierer,et al.  Brain Dynamics Underlying Training-Induced Improvement in Suppressing Inappropriate Action , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Simon van Gaal,et al.  Unconscious Activation of the Prefrontal No-Go Network , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociating Inhibition, Attention, and Response Control in the Frontoparietal Network Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Hold Your Horses: Impulsivity, Deep Brain Stimulation, and Medication in Parkinsonism , 2007, Science.

[34]  A. Aron From Reactive to Proactive and Selective Control: Developing a Richer Model for Stopping Inappropriate Responses , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.