Rats with Fimbria–Fornix Lesions Are Impaired in Path Integration: A Role for the Hippocampus in “Sense of Direction”

Animals can locate their present position in relation to a starting point and return to that starting point using cues generated by self-movement, a navigation strategy called dead-reckoning. Because contemporary research on spatial navigation suggests that some aspects of spatial navigation depend on the integrity of the hippocampal formation, whereas others do not, the present study examined whether dead-reckoning is hippocampally dependent. The task capitalized on the proclivity of foraging rats to carry large food pellets to a shelter for eating. Control rats and rats with fimbria–fornix (FF) lesions left a hidden burrow to search for one piece of food located somewhere on a circular table. The accuracy with which they returned to the burrow with the food was measured. In three experiments, rats received probe trials in which they (1) started from novel locations, (2) wore blindfolds to obscure visual cues, and (3) foraged under a condition in which surface cues, e.g., odors left by their outward searches, were displaced. Both sighted control and FF rats preferentially used visual cues for guidance when foraging from a familiar location. Control rats were accurate and FF rats were impaired in returning to novel starting locations (1) when sighted, (2) when blindfolded, and (3) when blindfolded in tests in which surface cues were displaced. These results, as well as detailed observations on the behavior of the animals, are consistent with the hypothesis that rats can use dead-reckoning to solve spatial problems, and this ability depends on the integrity of the hippocampal formation.

[1]  C. Darwin Origin of Certain Instincts , 1873, Nature.

[2]  John S. Brlow Inertial navigation as a basis for animal navigation , 1964 .

[3]  J. S. Barlow Inertial navigation as a basis for animal navigation. , 1964, Journal of theoretical biology.

[4]  J. O’Keefe Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat , 1976, Experimental Neurology.

[5]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[6]  C. Barnes Memory deficits associated with senescence: a neurophysiological and behavioral study in the rat. , 1979, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[7]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[8]  E. Batschelet Circular statistics in biology , 1981 .

[9]  N. McNaughton,et al.  Comparison between the behavioural effects of septal and hippocampal lesions: A review , 1983, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[10]  E. Spelke,et al.  Spatial knowledge in a young blind child , 1984, Cognition.

[11]  B. H. Bland The physiology and pharmacology of hippocampal formation theta rhythms , 1986, Progress in Neurobiology.

[12]  I Q Whishaw,et al.  Visits to starts, routes, and places by rats (Rattus norvegicus) in swimming pool navigation tasks. , 1986, Journal of comparative psychology.

[13]  A. Etienne The Control of Short-Distance Homing in the Golden Hamster , 1987 .

[14]  T. Robbins,et al.  Effects of excitotoxic lesions of the substantia innominata, ventral and dorsal globus pallidus on visual discrimination acquisition, performance and reversal in the rat , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[15]  R U Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  I. Whishaw Latent learning in a Swimming Pool Place Task by Rats: Evidence for the use of Associative and not Cognitive Mapping Processes , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[17]  D. Gaffan,et al.  Amnesia in man following transection of the fornix. A review. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  永福 智志 The Organization of Learning , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Cassel,et al.  Graft-induced behavioral recovery from subcallosal septohippocampal damage in rats depends on maturity stage of donor tissue , 1991, Neuroscience.

[20]  R Worden,et al.  Navigation by fragment fitting: A theory of hippocampal function , 1992, Hippocampus.

[21]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Spatial selectivity of unit activity in the hippocampal granular layer , 1993, Hippocampus.

[22]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Hippocampectomized monkeys can remember one place but not two , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  S. Mizumori,et al.  Directionally selective mnemonic properties of neurons in the lateral dorsal nucleus of the thalamus of rats , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  L. Jarrard On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.

[25]  I. Whishaw Activation, travel distance, and environmental change influence food carrying in rats with hippocampal, medial thalamic and septal lesions: Implications for studies on hoarding and theories of hippocampal function , 1993, Hippocampus.

[26]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Neurons responding to whole-body motion in the primate hippocampus , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  B. Will,et al.  Lesions of supracallosal or infracallosal hippocampal pathways in the rat: behavioral, neurochemical, and histochemical effects. , 1994, Behavioral and neural biology.

[28]  Ian Q. Whishaw,et al.  Food carrying: a new method for naturalistic studies of spontaneous and forced alternation , 1995, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[29]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Similarities vs. differences in place learning and circadian activity in rats after fimbria‐fornix section or ibotenate removal of hippocampal cells , 1995, Hippocampus.

[30]  P E Sharp,et al.  Influences of vestibular and visual motion information on the spatial firing patterns of hippocampal place cells , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Rats with fimbria-fornix lesions display a place response in a swimming pool: a dissociation between getting there and knowing where , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  J. Taube,et al.  Head direction cell activity monitored in a novel environment and during a cue conflict situation. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  J. Taube Head direction cells recorded in the anterior thalamic nuclei of freely moving rats , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  W E Skaggs,et al.  Deciphering the hippocampal polyglot: the hippocampus as a path integration system. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[35]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Evidence for extrahippocampal involvement in place learning and hippocampal involvement in path integration , 1996, Hippocampus.

[36]  P E Sharp,et al.  Visual and vestibular influences on head-direction cells in the anterior thalamus of the rat. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.

[37]  S. Wiener Spatial, behavioral and sensory correlates of hippocampal CA1 complex spike cell activity: Implications for information processing functions , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[38]  A S Etienne,et al.  Path integration in mammals and its interaction with visual landmarks. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[39]  RU Muller,et al.  The hippocampus as a cognitive graph , 1996, The Journal of general physiology.

[40]  J. Taube,et al.  Effects of repeated disorientation on the acquisition of spatial tasks in rats: dissociation between the appetitive radial arm maze and aversive water maze. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[41]  J. Taube,et al.  Head direction cells and episodic spatial information in rats without a hippocampus. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[42]  Ian Q Whishaw,et al.  Hippocampal lesions and path integration , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[43]  B L McNaughton,et al.  Path Integration and Cognitive Mapping in a Continuous Attractor Neural Network Model , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Piloting and dead reckoning dissociated by fimbria-fornix lesions in a rat food carrying task , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[45]  Patricia E. Sharp,et al.  Subicular cells generate similar spatial firing patterns in two geometrically and visually distinctive environments: Comparison with hippocampal place cells , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[46]  I. Whishaw Place Learning in Hippocampal Rats and the Path Integration Hypothesis , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.