Making connections among multiple graphical representations of fractions: sense-making competencies enhance perceptual fluency, but not vice versa

Prior research shows that representational competencies that enable students to use graphical representations to reason and solve tasks is key to learning in many science, technology, engineering, and mathematics domains. We focus on two types of representational competencies: (1) sense making of connections by verbally explaining how different representations map to one another, and (2) perceptual fluency that allows students to fast and effortlessly use perceptual features to make connections among representations. Because these different competencies are acquired via different types of learning processes, they require different types of instructional support: sense-making activities and fluency-building activities. In a prior experiment, we showed benefits for combining sense-making activities and fluency-building activities. In the current work, we test how to combine these two forms of instructional support, specifically, whether students should first work on sense-making activities or on fluency-building activities. This comparison allows us to investigate whether sense-making competencies enhance students’ acquisition of perceptual fluency (sense-making-first hypothesis) or whether perceptual fluency enhances students’ acquisition of sense-making competencies (fluency-first hypothesis). We conducted a lab experiment with 74 students from grades 3–5 working with an intelligent tutoring system for fractions. We assessed learning processes and learning outcomes related to representational competencies and domain knowledge. Overall, our results support the sense-making-first hypothesis, but not the fluency-first hypothesis.

[1]  Richard Lowe,et al.  Constructing a mental representation from an abstract technical diagram , 1993 .

[2]  S. Ainsworth DeFT: A Conceptual Framework for Considering Learning with Multiple Representations. , 2006 .

[3]  Vincent Aleven,et al.  A New Paradigm for Intelligent Tutoring Systems: Example-Tracing Tutors , 2009, Int. J. Artif. Intell. Educ..

[4]  D. Gentner,et al.  Learning and Transfer: A General Role for Analogical Encoding , 2003 .

[5]  Vincent Aleven,et al.  Why interactive learning environments can have it all: resolving design conflicts between competing goals , 2013, CHI.

[6]  Kathleen A. Cramer Using concrete models to build middle‐grade students understanding of functions , 2001 .

[7]  Keith S. Taber,et al.  Revisiting the chemistry triplet: drawing upon the nature of chemical knowledge and the psychology of learning to inform chemistry education , 2013 .

[8]  Ton de Jong,et al.  The effects of directive self-explanation prompts to support active processing of multiple representations in a simulation-based learning environment , 2011, J. Comput. Assist. Learn..

[9]  A. Su,et al.  The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics , 1932, The Mathematical Gazette.

[10]  R. Atkinson,et al.  Transitioning From Studying Examples to Solving Problems: Effects of Self-Explanation Prompts and Fading Worked-Out Steps. , 2003 .

[11]  David H. Uttal,et al.  Comprehending and Learning from ‘Visualizations’: A Developmental Perspective , 2008 .

[12]  Shaaron Ainsworth,et al.  The effects of self‐explaining when learning with text or diagrams , 2003 .

[13]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Cognitive Tutors: Lessons Learned , 1995 .

[14]  Hermann G. Ebner,et al.  Improving cross-content transfer in text processing by means of active graphical representation , 2003 .

[15]  Hans Spada,et al.  The Active Integration of Information during Learning with Dynamic and Interactive Visualisations , 2004 .

[16]  Mitchell J. Nathan,et al.  Representational disfluency in algebra: evidence from student gestures and speech , 2009 .

[17]  Ji Y. Son,et al.  Perceptual Learning Modules in Mathematics: Enhancing Students' Pattern Recognition, Structure Extraction, and Fluency , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[18]  T. Jong,et al.  Supporting students' learning with multiple representations in a dynamic simulation-based learning environment , 2006 .

[19]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  Reuniting perception and conception , 1998, Cognition.

[20]  Martha W. Alibali,et al.  Actions speak louder with words: The roles of action and pedagogical language for grounding mathematical proof , 2014 .

[21]  Vincent Aleven,et al.  Successful learning with multiple graphical representations and self-explanation prompts. , 2015 .

[22]  P. Kellman,et al.  Perceptual Learning, Cognition, and Expertise , 2013 .

[23]  Yash Patel,et al.  Using Multiple Representations to Build Conceptual Understanding in Science and Mathematics , 2014 .

[24]  Vincent Aleven,et al.  An effective metacognitive strategy: learning by doing and explaining with a computer-based Cognitive Tutor , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[25]  John R. Anderson The Architecture of Cognition , 1983 .

[26]  Richard Lowe,et al.  Selectivity in diagrams: reading beyond the lines , 1994 .

[27]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning: The Worked-Out Examples Principle in Multimedia Learning , 2005 .

[28]  T. Mueller Learning to Think Spatially , 2006 .

[29]  Vincent Aleven,et al.  Rule-Based Cognitive Modeling for Intelligent Tutoring Systems , 2010, Advances in Intelligent Tutoring Systems.

[30]  Erol Özçelik,et al.  An eye-tracking study of how color coding affects multimedia learning , 2009, Comput. Educ..

[31]  Percival G. Matthews,et al.  Fractions as percepts? Exploring cross-format distance effects for fractional magnitudes , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.

[32]  John K. Gilbert,et al.  Visualization: An Emergent Field of Practice and Enquiry in Science Education , 2008 .

[33]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[34]  John Airey,et al.  A disciplinary discourse perspective on university science learning: Achieving fluency in a critical constellation of modes , 2009 .

[35]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  Assisting self-explanation prompts are more effective than open prompts when learning with multiple representations , 2009 .

[36]  Loretta L. Jones,et al.  Molecular visualization in chemistry education: The role of multidisciplinary collaboration , 2005 .

[37]  Stephen J. Pape,et al.  The Role of Representation(s) in Developing Mathematical Understanding , 2001 .

[38]  Martina A. Rau,et al.  Conditions for the Effectiveness of Multiple Visual Representations in Enhancing STEM Learning , 2017 .

[39]  Robb Lindgren,et al.  Generating a learning stance through perspective-taking in a virtual environment , 2012, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[40]  Robert B. Kozma,et al.  Students Becoming Chemists: Developing Representationl Competence , 2005 .

[41]  Stellan Ohlsson,et al.  Computational Models of Skill Acquisition , 2008 .

[42]  Rolf Ploetzner,et al.  Supporting learning with interactive multimedia through active integration of representations , 2005 .

[43]  A. Glenberg,et al.  The illusion of knowing: Failure in the self-assessment of comprehension , 1982 .

[44]  Billie Eilam,et al.  Teaching, Learning, and Visual Literacy: The Dual Role of Visual Representation , 2012 .

[45]  Shaaron Ainsworth How Should We Evaluate Multimedia Learning Environments , 2008 .

[46]  E. Gibson Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development , 1969 .

[47]  Charalambos Y. Charalambous,et al.  Drawing on a Theoretical Model to Study Students’ Understandings of Fractions , 2007 .

[48]  P. Spirtes,et al.  Causation, prediction, and search , 1993 .

[49]  Tina Seufert Supporting Coherence Formation in Learning from Multiple Representations , 2003 .

[50]  Philip J. Kellman,et al.  Perceptual Learning and the Technology of Expertise: Studies in Fraction Learning and Algebra. , 2008 .

[51]  David Maxwell Chickering,et al.  Optimal Structure Identification With Greedy Search , 2002, J. Mach. Learn. Res..

[52]  S. Asli Özgün-Koca,et al.  Ninth Grade Students Studying the Movement of Fish to Learn about Linear Relationships: The Use of Video-Based Analysis Software in Mathematics Classrooms , 2008 .

[53]  John K. Gilbert,et al.  Visualization: A Metacognitive Skill in Science and Science Education , 2005 .

[54]  A. Dreher,et al.  Teachers Facing the Dilemma of Multiple Representations Being Aid and Obstacle for Learning: Evaluations of Tasks and Theme-Specific Noticing , 2015 .

[55]  K. Anders Ericsson Perceptual and Memory Processes in the Acquisition of Expert Performance: The EPAM Model , 2014 .

[56]  P. Shah,et al.  Exploring visuospatial thinking in chemistry learning , 2004 .

[57]  Zachary A. Pardos,et al.  How Should Intelligent Tutoring Systems Sequence Multiple Graphical Representations of Fractions? A Multi-Methods Study , 2013, International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education.

[58]  Shaaron Ainsworth,et al.  Examining the Effects of Different Multiple Representational Systems in Learning Primary Mathematics , 2002 .

[59]  Wolfgang Schnotz,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning: An Integrated Model of Text and Picture Comprehension , 2005 .

[60]  Thomas E. Kieren,et al.  Rational and fractional numbers: From quotient fields to recursive understanding , 1993 .

[61]  K. VanLehn The Relative Effectiveness of Human Tutoring, Intelligent Tutoring Systems, and Other Tutoring Systems , 2011 .

[62]  Barbara Y. White,et al.  Making Their Own Connections: Students' Understanding of Multiple Models in Basic Electricity , 1999 .

[63]  Xiaojin Zhu,et al.  Humans Learn Using Manifolds, Reluctantly , 2010, NIPS.

[64]  J. Deloache Dual representation and young children's use of scale models. , 2000, Child development.

[65]  Sibel Kazak,et al.  Saying More than You Know in Instructional Settings , 2011 .

[66]  Kenneth R. Koedinger,et al.  A Data Repository for the EDM Community: The PSLC DataShop , 2010 .

[67]  A. Renkl,et al.  Instructional Aids to Support a Conceptual Understanding of Multiple Representations. , 2009 .

[68]  E. Gibson Perceptual Learning in Development: Some Basic Concepts , 2000 .

[69]  J. R. Landis,et al.  The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. , 1977, Biometrics.

[70]  Cheryl I. Johnson,et al.  Applying the self-explanation principle to multimedia learning in a computer-based game-like environment , 2010, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[71]  D. Gentner Structure‐Mapping: A Theoretical Framework for Analogy* , 1983 .

[72]  Vincent Aleven,et al.  Supporting Students in Making Sense of Connections and in Becoming Perceptually Fluent in Making Connections Among Multiple Graphical Representations , 2017 .

[73]  Albert T. Corbett,et al.  The Knowledge-Learning-Instruction Framework: Bridging the Science-Practice Chasm to Enhance Robust Student Learning , 2012, Cogn. Sci..

[74]  Vincent Aleven,et al.  Example-Tracing Tutors: Intelligent Tutor Development for Non-programmers , 2016, International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education.

[75]  F. Paas,et al.  Uncovering the problem-solving process: cued retrospective reporting versus concurrent and retrospective reporting. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[76]  Timothy J. Nokes-Malach,et al.  Effectiveness of holistic mental model confrontation in driving conceptual change , 2012 .

[77]  Hans Spada,et al.  Acquiring knowledge in science and mathematics : the use of multiple representations in technology based learning environments , 1998 .

[78]  Tina Seufert,et al.  Cognitive load and the format of instructional aids for coherence formation , 2006 .

[79]  Jennifer L. Chiu,et al.  Evidence for effective uses of dynamic visualisations in science curriculum materials , 2015 .

[80]  Daniel Bodemer,et al.  External and mental referencing of multiple representations , 2006, Comput. Hum. Behav..