Positive and negative compatibility effects
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[2] Elizabeth L Bjork,et al. On the nature of input channels in visual processing. , 1977, Psychological review.
[3] D. Navon. Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.
[4] H E Egeth,et al. Interference in letter identification: A test of feature-specific inhibition , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.
[5] Howard E. Egeth,et al. Conceptual and perceptual components of interletter inhibition. , 1981 .
[6] H. Philips,et al. A Psychophysiological Investigation of Tension Headache , 1982, Headache.
[7] Jeff Miller,et al. Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[8] D Navon,et al. How Many Trees Does it Take to Make a Forest? , 1983, Perception.
[9] C. Eriksen,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .
[10] Global precedence in attended and nonattended objects. , 1988 .
[11] P. Merikle,et al. Global precedence in attended and nonattended objects. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[12] Lise Paquet,et al. L'attention sélective : l'effet du critère de sélection , 1988 .
[13] Jeff F. Miller. The flanker compatibility effect as a function of visual angle, attentional focus, visual transients, and perceptual load: A search for boundary conditions , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[14] A. Kramer,et al. Perceptual organization and focused attention: The role of objects and proximity in visual processing , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[15] G. Baylis,et al. Visual parsing and response competition: The effect of grouping factors , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[16] J. Greaney,et al. The order of visual processing: Top-down, bottom-up, middle-out, or none of these? , 1992 .
[17] L. Paquet. Global and local processing in nonattended objects: a failure to induce local processing dominance. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] Efficient filtering of irrelevant global and local information when target level and location are random , 1993 .
[19] Gregory Bock,et al. Higher-order processing in the visual system , 1994 .
[20] M. Farah,et al. Does visual attention select objects or locations? , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[21] Selective attention to global and local structure of objects: Alternative measures of nontarget processing , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[22] H C Nothdurft,et al. Common properties of visual segmentation. , 1994, Ciba Foundation symposium.
[23] C. Leeuwen,et al. Stroop can occur without Garner interference: Strategic and mandatory influences in multidimensional stimuli , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] S. Kornblum,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] N. Lavie. Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] G. Logan,et al. Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention , 1996 .
[27] S. Yantis. 2. Attentional capture in vision , 1996 .
[28] M. Coles,et al. The dimensionality of the flanker compatibility effect: A psychophysiological analysis , 1997, Psychological research.
[29] N. Lavie,et al. On the Efficiency of Visual Selective Attention: Efficient Visual Search Leads to Inefficient Distractor Rejection , 1997 .
[30] J Fagot,et al. Processing of global and local visual information and hemispheric specialization in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papio papio). , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.