How Partisan Crowds Affect News Evaluation

Social influence is ubiquitous in politics and online social media. Here we explore how social signals from partisan crowds influence people’s evaluations of political news. For example, are liberals easily persuaded by a liberal crowd, while resisting the influence of conservative crowds? We designed a largescale online experiment (N=1,000) to test how politically-annotated social signals affect participants’ opinions. In times rife with misinformation and polarization, our findings are optimistic: the mechanism of social influence works across political lines, that is, liberals are reliably influenced by majority-Republican crowds and vice versa. At the same time, we replicate findings showing that people are inclined to discard news claims that are inconsistent with their political views. Considering that people show negative reactions to politically dissonant news but not to social signals that oppose their views, we point to the possibility of depolarizing social rating systems.

[1]  Jon Kleinberg,et al.  Differences in the mechanics of information diffusion across topics: idioms, political hashtags, and complex contagion on twitter , 2011, WWW.

[2]  Lada A. Adamic,et al.  Social influence and the diffusion of user-created content , 2009, EC '09.

[3]  Lada A. Adamic,et al.  Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on Facebook , 2015, Science.

[4]  Cosma Rohilla Shalizi,et al.  Homophily and Contagion Are Generically Confounded in Observational Social Network Studies , 2010, Sociological methods & research.

[5]  Jacob Ratkiewicz,et al.  Political Polarization on Twitter , 2011, ICWSM.

[6]  Carey K. Morewedge,et al.  Social defaults : Observed choices become choice defaults , 2014 .

[7]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  M. Macy,et al.  Complex Contagions and the Weakness of Long Ties1 , 2007, American Journal of Sociology.

[9]  M. Deutsch,et al.  A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgement. , 1955, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[10]  Charles S. Taber,et al.  Motivated Skepticism in the Evaluation of Political Beliefs , 2006 .

[11]  Chengkai Li,et al.  Toward Automated Fact-Checking: Detecting Check-worthy Factual Claims by ClaimBuster , 2017, KDD.

[12]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Situating Social Influence Processes: Dynamic, Multidirectional Flows of Influence Within Social Networks , 2007, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[13]  Matthew J. Salganik,et al.  Experimental Study of Inequality and Unpredictability in an Artificial Cultural Market , 2006, Science.

[14]  Larry M. Bartels Beyond the Running Tally: Partisan Bias in Political Perceptions , 2002 .

[15]  Sean J. Westwood,et al.  Selective Exposure in the Age of Social Media , 2014, Commun. Res..

[16]  B. Nyhan,et al.  When Corrections Fail: The Persistence of Political Misperceptions , 2010 .

[17]  J. Druckman,et al.  The Nature and Origins of Misperceptions: Understanding False and Unsupported Beliefs About Politics , 2017 .

[18]  John Riedl,et al.  Is seeing believing?: how recommender system interfaces affect users' opinions , 2003, CHI '03.

[19]  Jay D. Hmielowski,et al.  Facebook news and (de)polarization: reinforcing spirals in the 2016 US election , 2018 .

[20]  Michael Macy,et al.  Opinion cascades and the unpredictability of partisan polarization , 2019, Science Advances.

[21]  M. McPherson,et al.  Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks , 2001 .

[22]  Sinan Aral,et al.  The spread of true and false news online , 2018, Science.

[23]  M. Macy,et al.  Local Convergence and Global Diversity: From Interpersonal to Social , 2016 .

[24]  Jonah Berger,et al.  Alone in a crowd of sheep: asymmetric perceptions of conformity and their roots in an introspection illusion. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  Taylor W. Brown,et al.  Exposure to opposing views on social media can increase political polarization , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Chrysanthos Dellarocas,et al.  The Digitization of Word-of-Mouth: Promise and Challenges of Online Feedback Mechanisms , 2003, Manag. Sci..

[27]  A. Gelman,et al.  Partisans without Constraint: Political Polarization and Trends in American Public Opinion. , 2008, AJS; American journal of sociology.

[28]  C. D. Vreese,et al.  Political Parties, Motivated Reasoning, and Issue Framing Effects , 2010 .

[29]  Sean J. Taylor,et al.  Social Influence Bias: A Randomized Experiment , 2013, Science.

[30]  Thorsten Joachims,et al.  Recommendations as Treatments: Debiasing Learning and Evaluation , 2016, ICML.

[31]  C. Sunstein,et al.  Epistemic spillovers: Learning others’ political views reduces the ability to assess and use their expertise in nonpolitical domains , 2019, Cognition.

[32]  N. Stroud Polarization and Partisan Selective Exposure , 2010 .

[33]  S. Asch Opinions and Social Pressure , 1955, Nature.

[34]  David G. Rand,et al.  Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning , 2019, Cognition.

[35]  Matthew J. Lindberg,et al.  Feeling validated versus being correct: a meta-analysis of selective exposure to information. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.

[36]  H. Simons,et al.  Similarity, credibility, and attitude change: A review and a theory. , 1970 .

[37]  Siddharth Suri,et al.  Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[38]  Ethan Porter,et al.  The Elusive Backfire Effect: Mass Attitudes’ Steadfast Factual Adherence , 2019 .

[39]  Randall L. Calvert,et al.  The Value of Biased Information: A Rational Choice Model of Political Advice , 1985, The Journal of Politics.

[40]  D. Hopkins,et al.  Political polarization in American politics , 2015 .

[41]  D. Kahan,et al.  Cultural cognition of scientific consensus , 2011 .

[42]  Daniel B. Wright,et al.  Beyond unusual? Examining the role of attention in the weapon focus effect , 2007 .

[43]  E. Berscheid,et al.  Opinion change and communicator-communicatee similarity and dissimilarity. , 1966 .

[44]  R. Bond,et al.  Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis of studies using Asch's (1952b, 1956) line judgment task. , 1996 .

[45]  L. Festinger,et al.  A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 2017 .

[46]  Eli Pariser,et al.  The Filter Bubble: How the New Personalized Web Is Changing What We Read and How We Think , 2012 .

[47]  A. Coppock Generalizing from Survey Experiments Conducted on Mechanical Turk: A Replication Approach , 2018, Political Science Research and Methods.

[48]  Philip D. Waggoner,et al.  Are samples drawn from Mechanical Turk valid for research on political ideology? , 2015 .

[49]  R. Kelly Garrett,et al.  Electoral Consequences of Political Rumors: Motivated Reasoning, Candidate Rumors, and Vote Choice during the 2008 U.S. Presidential Election , 2014 .

[50]  The Role of Source and Expressive Responding in Political News Evaluation , 2018 .

[51]  Austin Lee Nichols,et al.  The Good-Subject Effect: Investigating Participant Demand Characteristics , 2008, The Journal of general psychology.

[52]  H. Kelman PROCESSES OF OPINION CHANGE , 1961 .