Promoting rotational-invariance in object recognition despite experience with only a single view
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] I. Biederman,et al. The Pigeon's Recognition of Drawings of Depth-Rotated Stimuli , 1996 .
[2] Tomaso Poggio,et al. Models of object recognition , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[3] Tomaso Poggio,et al. Fast Readout of Object Identity from Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2005, Science.
[4] Edward A Wasserman,et al. Missing the Forest for the Trees , 2010, Psychological science.
[5] I Biederman,et al. Seeing things from a different angle: the pigeon's recognition of single geons rotated in depth. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[6] Lloyd J. Frei,et al. Recent advances in operant conditioning technology: A versatile and affordable computerized touchscreen system , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[7] Edward A. Wasserman,et al. View-invariance learning in object recognition by pigeons depends on error-driven associative learning processes , 2012, Vision Research.
[8] Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al. Slow Feature Analysis: Unsupervised Learning of Invariances , 2002, Neural Computation.
[9] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Prior experience of rotation is not required for recognizing objects seen from different angles , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[10] David D. Cox,et al. 'Breaking' position-invariant object recognition , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[11] David D. Cox,et al. What response properties do individual neurons need to underlie position and clutter "invariant" object recognition? , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[12] Edmund T. Rolls,et al. Learning invariant object recognition in the visual system with continuous transformations , 2006, Biological Cybernetics.
[13] Fabian A. Soto,et al. Visual object categorization in birds and primates: Integrating behavioral, neurobiological, and computational evidence within a “general process” framework , 2011, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.
[14] I. Biederman,et al. Inferior Temporal Neurons Show Greater Sensitivity to Nonaccidental than to Metric Shape Differences , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[15] Charles F Stevens. Preserving properties of object shape by computations in primary visual cortex. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[16] Fabian A. Soto,et al. A category-overshadowing effect in pigeons: support for the Common Elements Model of object categorization learning. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[17] I. Biederman,et al. Shape Tuning in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[18] Isabel Gauthier,et al. Three-dimensional object recognition is viewpoint dependent , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[19] Tomaso Poggio,et al. Trade-Off between Object Selectivity and Tolerance in Monkey Inferotemporal Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[20] I. Biederman,et al. Effects of varying stimulus size on object recognition in pigeons. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[21] E. Rolls,et al. View-invariant representations of familiar objects by neurons in the inferior temporal visual cortex. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.
[22] Fabian A. Soto,et al. Error-driven learning in visual categorization and object recognition: a common-elements model. , 2010, Psychological review.
[23] A Friedman,et al. The effect of distinctive parts on recognition of depth-rotated objects by pigeons (Columba livia) and humans. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[24] I. Biederman. Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.
[25] P. Földiák,et al. Forming sparse representations by local anti-Hebbian learning , 1990, Biological Cybernetics.
[26] Edward A. Wasserman,et al. Pigeons and humans are more sensitive to nonaccidental than to metric changes in visual objects , 2008, Behavioural Processes.
[27] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[28] H. Bülthoff,et al. Effects of temporal association on recognition memory , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[29] David J. Field,et al. Wavelets, vision and the statistics of natural scenes , 1999, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.
[30] Dwight J. Kravitz,et al. How position dependent is visual object recognition? , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[31] R. Vogels,et al. Spatial sensitivity of macaque inferior temporal neurons , 2000, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[32] Fabian A. Soto,et al. Mechanisms of object recognition: what we have learned from pigeons , 2014, Front. Neural Circuits.
[33] P. Schyns,et al. Nonaccidental Properties Underlie Shape Recognition in Mammalian and Nonmammalian Vision , 2007, Current Biology.
[34] Robbe L. T. Goris,et al. Invariance in Visual Object Recognition Requires Training: A Computational Argument , 2009, Frontiers in neuroscience.
[35] I. Biederman,et al. Recognition-by-components: A bird's eye view. , 2012 .
[36] James J. DiCarlo,et al. Unsupervised Natural Experience Rapidly Alters Invariant Object Representation in Visual Cortex , 2008, Science.
[37] A. N. Bowers,et al. Visual circuits of the avian telencephalon: evolutionary implications , 1999, Behavioural Brain Research.
[38] Keiji Tanaka,et al. View‐invariant object recognition ability develops after discrimination, not mere exposure, at several viewing angles , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.