Is intracranial volume a suitable proxy for brain reserve?

BackgroundBrain reserve is a concept introduced to explain why Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients with a greater brain volume prior to onset of pathology generally have better clinical outcomes. In this review, we provide a historical background of the emergence of brain reserve and discuss several aspects that need further clarification, including the dynamic or static nature of the concept and its underlying mechanisms and clinical effect. We then describe how brain reserve has been operationalized over the years, and critically evaluate the use of intracranial volume (ICV) as the most widely used proxy for brain reserve. Furthermore, we perform a meta-analysis showing that ICV is associated with higher cognitive performance after adjusting for the presence and amount of pathology. Although we acknowledge its imperfections, we conclude that the use of ICV as a proxy for brain reserve is currently warranted. However, further development of more optimal measures of brain reserve as well as a more clearly defined theoretical framework is essential.

[1]  M. Gearing,et al.  Dendritic spines provide cognitive resilience against Alzheimer's disease , 2017, Annals of neurology.

[2]  E. Mori,et al.  Premorbid brain size as a determinant of reserve capacity against intellectual decline in Alzheimer's disease. , 1997, The American journal of psychiatry.

[3]  Hannah R Rothstein,et al.  A basic introduction to fixed‐effect and random‐effects models for meta‐analysis , 2010, Research synthesis methods.

[4]  Nick C Fox,et al.  The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease , 2011, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[5]  J. Giedd,et al.  Brain development in children and adolescents: Insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[6]  Khader M Hasan,et al.  Human brain atlas‐based volumetry and relaxometry: Application to healthy development and natural aging , 2010, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[7]  Lars Johansson,et al.  Intracranial volume estimated with commonly used methods could introduce bias in studies including brain volume measurements , 2013, NeuroImage.

[8]  A. Sierra,et al.  Lifestyle Shapes the Dialogue between Environment, Microglia, and Adult Neurogenesis. , 2016, ACS chemical neuroscience.

[9]  D. Mathalon,et al.  A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. , 1994, Archives of neurology.

[10]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume , 2004, NeuroImage.

[11]  E B Larson,et al.  Head Circumference as a Measure of Cognitive Reserve , 1996, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[12]  H. Braak,et al.  The preclinical phase of the pathological process underlying sporadic Alzheimer's disease. , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  Ana Navarrete,et al.  Energetics and the evolution of human brain size , 2011, Nature.

[14]  E. Chang,et al.  Human hippocampal neurogenesis drops sharply in children to undetectable levels in adults , 2018, Nature.

[15]  Eileen Luders,et al.  Gender differences in cortical complexity , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  J. P. Rushton,et al.  Whole Brain Size and General Mental Ability: A Review , 2009, The International journal of neuroscience.

[17]  F. Gage,et al.  Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus , 1998, Nature Medicine.

[18]  C. Lebel,et al.  Longitudinal Development of Human Brain Wiring Continues from Childhood into Adulthood , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Hagen B. Huttner,et al.  Dynamics of Hippocampal Neurogenesis in Adult Humans , 2013, Cell.

[20]  P. Satz Brain reserve capacity on symptom onset after brain injury: A formulation and review of evidence for threshold theory. , 1993 .

[21]  C. Lowe Effect of Mothers' Smoking Habits on Birth Weight of their Children , 1959 .

[22]  Allan R. Jones,et al.  An anatomically comprehensive atlas of the adult human brain transcriptome , 2012, Nature.

[23]  F. Verhage Intelligentie en leeftijd : onderzoek bij Nederlanders van twaalf tot zevenenzeventig jaar , 1964 .

[24]  Joanna M. Wardlaw,et al.  Estimated maximal and current brain volume predict cognitive ability in old age , 2013, Neurobiology of Aging.

[25]  Sébastien Ourselin,et al.  Head size, age and gender adjustment in MRI studies: a necessary nuisance? , 2010, NeuroImage.

[26]  Head circumference of rural Nigerian children--the effect of malnutrition on brain growth. , 1997, The Central African journal of medicine.

[27]  C. Jack,et al.  Age-specific population frequencies of cerebral β-amyloidosis and neurodegeneration among people with normal cognitive function aged 50–89 years: a cross-sectional study , 2014, The Lancet Neurology.

[28]  W. M. van der Flier,et al.  Differential effects of cognitive reserve and brain reserve on cognition in Alzheimer disease , 2018, Neurology.

[29]  M. Boldrini,et al.  Human Hippocampal Neurogenesis Persists throughout Aging. , 2018, Cell stem cell.

[30]  D. Weinberger,et al.  Genetic variability of human brain size and cortical gyral patterns. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  F. Gage,et al.  Human Adult Neurogenesis: Evidence and Remaining Questions. , 2018, Cell stem cell.

[32]  H. Christensen,et al.  Education, atrophy, and cognitive change in an epidemiological sample in early old age. , 2009, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.

[33]  D. Snowdon,et al.  Height as a marker of childhood development and late-life cognitive function: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. , 1998, Pediatrics.

[34]  H. Cameron,et al.  Environmental Control of Adult Neurogenesis: From Hippocampal Homeostasis to Behavior and Disease , 2014, Neural plasticity.

[35]  D. Bowers,et al.  Proof of principle: Transformation approach alters caudate nucleus volume and structure-function associations , 2014, Brain Imaging and Behavior.

[36]  E G Tangalos,et al.  Total intracranial volume: Normative values and lack of association with Alzheimer’s disease , 2002, Neurology.

[37]  D. Spencer,et al.  Imaging synaptic density in the living human brain , 2016, Science Translational Medicine.

[38]  Y. Stern,et al.  Cognitive reserve in aging. , 2011, Current Alzheimer research.

[39]  R. Katzman.,et al.  Clinical, pathological, and neurochemical changes in dementia: A subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques , 1988, Annals of neurology.

[40]  Nick C Fox,et al.  The Diagnosis of Mild Cognitive Impairment due to Alzheimer’s Disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association Workgroups on Diagnostic Guidelines for Alzheimer’s Disease , 2011 .

[41]  R. Lynn A NUTRITION THEORY OF THE SECULAR INCREASES IN INTELLIGENCE; POSITIVE CORRELATIONS BETWEEN HEIGHT, HEAD SIZE AND IQ , 1989 .

[42]  R. Kahn,et al.  A genetic analysis of brain volumes and IQ in children , 2009 .

[43]  Nick C Fox,et al.  Pitfalls in the Use of Voxel-Based Morphometry as a Biomarker: Examples from Huntington Disease , 2010, American Journal of Neuroradiology.

[44]  J. Trojanowski,et al.  Cognitive and functional resilience despite molecular evidence of Alzheimer’s disease pathology , 2013, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[45]  B. Winblad,et al.  Intracranial volume in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia: evidence for brain reserve? , 2004 .

[46]  W. M. van der Flier,et al.  A neuroimaging approach to capture cognitive reserve: Application to Alzheimer's disease , 2017, Human brain mapping.

[47]  C. Jack,et al.  A large-scale comparison of cortical thickness and volume methods for measuring Alzheimer's disease severity , 2016, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[48]  G. Chételat,et al.  Cognitive reserve and lifestyle: moving towards preclinical Alzheimer’s disease , 2015, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[49]  Charles DeCarli,et al.  Maximal brain size remains an important predictor of cognition in old age, independent of current brain pathology , 2012, Neurobiology of Aging.

[50]  T. Paus,et al.  Brain size and folding of the human cerebral cortex. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  D. Y. Lee,et al.  Prevalence of cerebral amyloid pathology in persons without dementia: a meta-analysis. , 2015, JAMA.

[52]  S. Barger,et al.  Why neurons die: Cell death in the nervous system , 1998, The Anatomical record.

[53]  G. Schellenberg,et al.  Head circumference and incident Alzheimer’s disease , 2001, Neurology.

[54]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  A theoretical framework for the study of adult cognitive plasticity. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[55]  M. Roth,et al.  The association of clinical and neurological findings and its bearing on the classification and aetiology of Alzheimer's disease. , 1986, British medical bulletin.

[56]  J. Kaye,et al.  Cognitive impairment risk , 2009, Neurology.

[57]  M. Glymour,et al.  Conceptual and Measurement Challenges in Research on Cognitive Reserve , 2011, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[58]  Yaakov Stern,et al.  An approach to studying the neural correlates of reserve , 2017, Brain Imaging and Behavior.

[59]  Y. Stern Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease , 2012, The Lancet Neurology.

[60]  I. Deary,et al.  What provides cerebral reserve? , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[61]  C. Siao,et al.  Genetic Variant BDNF (Val66Met) Polymorphism Alters Anxiety-Related Behavior , 2006, Science.

[62]  Peter Kochunov,et al.  Heritability of brain volume, surface area and shape: An MRI study in an extended pedigree of baboons , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[63]  L. Wahlund,et al.  Structural correlates of mild cognitive impairment , 2004, Neurobiology of Aging.

[64]  J. Morris,et al.  The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease , 2011, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[65]  Jennifer L. Whitwell,et al.  Accurate automatic estimation of total intracranial volume: A nuisance variable with less nuisance , 2015, NeuroImage.

[66]  M N Rossor,et al.  Intracranial volume and Alzheimer disease: evidence against the cerebral reserve hypothesis. , 2000, Archives of neurology.

[67]  D. Tate,et al.  Intracranial volume and dementia: Some evidence in support of the cerebral reserve hypothesis , 2011, Brain Research.

[68]  P. Schofield Alzheimer's Disease and Brain Reserve , 1999 .

[69]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.