Learning to navigate: Experience versus maps

People use "route knowledge" to navigate to targets along familiar routes and "survey knowledge" to determine (by pointing, for example) a target's metric location. We show that both root in separate memories of the same environment: participants navigating through their home city relied on representations and reference frames different from those they used when doing a matched survey task. Tübingen residents recalled their way along a familiar route to a distant target while located in a photorealistic virtual 3D model of Tübingen, indicating their route decisions on a keyboard. Participants had previously done a survey task (pointing) using the same start points and targets. Errors and response latencies observed in route recall were completely unrelated to errors and latencies in pointing. This suggests participants employed different and independent representations for each task. Further, participants made fewer routing errors when asked to respond from a horizontal walking perspective rather than a constant aerial perspective. This suggests that instead of the single reference, north-up frame (similar to a conventional map) they used in the survey task, participants employed different, and most probably multiple, reference frames learned from "on the ground" navigating experience. The implication is that, within their everyday environment, people use map or navigation-based knowledge according to which best suits the task.

[1]  T. McNamara,et al.  Orientation and perspective dependence in route and survey learning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[2]  Gabriele Janzen,et al.  Memory for object location and route direction in virtual large-scale space , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  Eliahu Stern,et al.  Levels of Spatial Knowledge and Urban Travel Modeling , 2010 .

[4]  D. R. Montello,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from direct experience in the environment: Individual differences in the development of metric knowledge and the integration of separately learned places , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  Jean-Arcady Meyer,et al.  BIOLOGICALLY BASED ARTIFICIAL NAVIGATION SYSTEMS: REVIEW AND PROSPECTS , 1997, Progress in Neurobiology.

[6]  Gerhard Strube,et al.  Adaptivity of wayfinding strategies in a multi-building ensemble: The effects of spatial structure, task requirements, and metric information , 2009 .

[7]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  B Tversky,et al.  Descriptions and depictions of environments , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[9]  Tobias Meilinger,et al.  Is the Map in Our Head Oriented North? , 2012, Psychological science.

[10]  Vladimir I. Levenshtein,et al.  Binary codes capable of correcting deletions, insertions, and reversals , 1965 .

[11]  Richard B. Chase,et al.  THE ACQUISITION OF ROUTE AND SURVEY KNOWLEDGE FROM COMPUTER MODELS , 1999 .

[12]  E. Hutchins Understanding practice: Learning to navigate , 1993 .

[13]  H. Taylor,et al.  Goal-specific influences on the representation of spatial perspective , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[14]  Tobias Meilinger,et al.  The Network of Reference Frames Theory: A Synthesis of Graphs and Cognitive Maps , 2008, Spatial Cognition.

[15]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  From Isovists via Mental Representations to Behaviour: First Steps toward Closing the Causal Chain , 2007 .

[16]  Jonathan W. Kelly,et al.  Sensorimotor alignment effects in the learning environment and in novel environments. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  Shannon Dawn Moeser Cognitive Mapping in a Complex Building , 1988 .

[18]  J. Piaget,et al.  Child's Conception Of Geometry , 1960 .

[19]  T. McNamara,et al.  Human Spatial Memory and Navigation , 2008 .

[20]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Changing perspective within and across environments , 2003, Cognition.

[21]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[22]  D. Holding,et al.  Acquisition of Route Network Knowledge by Males and Females , 1989 .

[23]  D. Appleyard Styles and Methods of Structuring a City , 1970 .

[24]  Tobias Meilinger,et al.  Local and Global Reference Frames for Environmental Spaces , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[25]  Markus Knauff,et al.  Up the down staircase : Wayfinding strategies in multi-level buildings , 2006 .

[26]  M. Mehta Human spatial memory , 2010 .

[27]  Hanspeter A. Mallot,et al.  Embodied spatial cognition: Biological and artificial systems , 2009, Image Vis. Comput..

[28]  B. Poucet Spatial cognitive maps in animals: new hypotheses on their structure and neural mechanisms. , 1993, Psychological review.

[29]  Robert Lloyd,et al.  Cognitive Maps: Encoding and Decoding Information , 1989 .

[30]  Michael Tlauka,et al.  Orientation specificity occurs in both small- and large-scale imagined routes presented as verbal descriptions. , 1999 .

[31]  Donald Appleyard,et al.  Why Buildings Are Known , 1969 .

[32]  Christoph Hölscher,et al.  Taxonomy of Human Wayfinding Tasks: A Knowledge-Based Approach , 2009, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[33]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[34]  D. R. Montello,et al.  Integrating Knowledge of Vertically Aligned Large-Scale Spaces , 1993 .

[35]  Reginald G. Golledge,et al.  Path Selection and Route Preference in Human Navigation: A Progress Report , 1995, COSIT.

[36]  Hanspeter A. Mallot,et al.  Mental Travel Primes Place Orientation in Spatial Recall , 2012, Spatial Cognition.