Stereomotion perception for a monocularly camouflaged stimulus.

Under usual circumstances, motion in depth is associated with conventional stereomotion cues: a change in disparity and differences between object velocities in each monocular image. However, occasionally these cues are unavailable due to the fact that in one eye the object may be occluded by, or camouflaged against appropriately positioned binocular objects. We report two experiments concerned with stereomotion perception under conditions of monocular camouflage. In Experiment 1, the visible half-image of a monocularly camouflaged object translated laterally. In this binocular context, percepts of lateral motion and motion in depth were equally consistent with the stimulus. Subjects perceived an oblique trajectory of 3D motion, compared to the more direct 3D trajectory experienced for binocularly matched stimuli. In Experiment 2, the perceived velocity of stereomotion was assessed. Again, for the stimulus used in Experiment 1, perceived stereomotion speed was lower than that for matched stimuli. However, when additional background objects were present, tightening the ecological constraints, perceived stereomotion velocity was often equivalent to that for matched stimuli. These results demonstrate for the first time that the motion of a monocularly camouflaged object can result in the perception of stereomotion, and that the perceived trajectory and speed are influenced by the ecological constraints of binocular geometry.

[1]  Charles Wheatstone,et al.  Contributions to the Physiology of Vision. , 1837 .

[2]  C. Wheatstone XVIII. Contributions to the physiology of vision. —Part the first. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision , 1962, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.

[3]  Charles Wheatstone On some remarkable and hitherto unobserved phenomena of binocular vision. , 1962 .

[4]  C W Tyler,et al.  Stereoscopic Depth Movement: Two Eyes Less Sensitive than One , 1971, Science.

[5]  D Regan,et al.  Some dynamic features of depth perception. , 1973, Vision research.

[6]  D. Regan,et al.  Binocular and monocular stimuli for motion in depth: Changing-disparity and changing-size feed the same motion-in-depth stage , 1979, Vision Research.

[7]  A. Norcia,et al.  Temporal frequency limits for stereoscopic apparent motion processes , 1984, Vision Research.

[8]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Da vinci stereopsis: Depth and subjective occluding contours from unpaired image points , 1990, Vision Research.

[9]  D. Regan,et al.  Binocular correlates of the direction of motion in depth , 1993, Vision Research.

[10]  B. G. Cumming,et al.  Binocular mechanisms for detecting motion-in-depth , 1994, Vision Research.

[11]  Barton L. Anderson,et al.  The role of partial occlusion in stereopsis , 1994, Nature.

[12]  I P Howard,et al.  Depth from Binocular Rivalry without Spatial Disparity , 1995, Perception.

[13]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Speed discrimination of motion-in-depth using binocular cues , 1995, Vision Research.

[14]  D. Regan,et al.  Cyclopean Discrimination Thresholds for the Direction and Speed of Motion in Depth , 1996, Vision Research.

[15]  G. Nyman,et al.  Depth Asymmetry in da Vinci Stereopsis , 1996, Vision Research.

[16]  D. Regan,et al.  Cyclopean motion perception produced by Oscillations of size, disparity and location , 1996, Vision Research.

[17]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Quantitative depth for a phantom surface can be based on cyclopean occlusion cues alone , 1999, Vision Research.

[18]  G. Mather,et al.  Perceived speed of motion in depth is reduced in the periphery , 2000, Vision Research.

[19]  Hirohisa Yaguchi,et al.  Motion in depth based on inter-ocular velocity differences , 2000, Vision Research.

[20]  K. Brooks,et al.  Stereomotion Speed Perception is Contrast Dependent , 2001, Perception.

[21]  Jukka Häkkinen,et al.  Phantom surface captures stereopsis , 2001, Vision Research.

[22]  Kevin R Brooks,et al.  Interocular velocity difference contributes to stereomotion speed perception. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[23]  Kevin R Brooks,et al.  Monocular motion adaptation affects the perceived trajectory of stereomotion. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Hiroshi Ono,et al.  Content and context of monocular regions determine perceived depth in random dot, unpaired background and phantom stereograms , 2002, Vision Research.

[25]  Peter Lennie,et al.  Binocular integration of partially occluded surfaces , 2002, Vision Research.

[26]  Michael J Pianta,et al.  Paired and unpaired features can be equally effective in human depth perception , 2003, Vision Research.

[27]  Barbara J Gillam,et al.  Monocular gap stereopsis: manipulation of the outer edge disparity and the shape of the gap , 2003, Vision Research.

[28]  Michael Cook,et al.  Depth of monocular elements in a binocular scene: the conditions for da Vinci stereopsis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  Leland S Stone,et al.  Stereomotion speed perception: contributions from both changing disparity and interocular velocity difference over a range of relative disparities. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[30]  Bart Farell,et al.  Seeing motion in depth using inter-ocular velocity differences , 2005, Vision Research.

[31]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Using visual direction in three-dimensional motion perception , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  B. Gillam,et al.  How Configurations of Binocular Disparity Determine Whether Stereoscopic Slant or Stereoscopic Occlusion is Seen , 2005, Perception.

[33]  Martin Lages Bayesian models of binocular 3-D motion perception. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[34]  Leland S Stone,et al.  Stereomotion suppression and the perception of speed: accuracy and precision as a function of 3D trajectory. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[35]  Bart Farell,et al.  Motion in depth from interocular velocity differences revealed by differential motion aftereffect , 2006, Vision Research.

[36]  Leland S Stone,et al.  Spatial scale of stereomotion speed processing. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[37]  Barbara J Gillam,et al.  The swinging doors of perception: stereomotion without binocular matching. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[38]  K. R. Brooks,et al.  Quantitative perceived depth from sequential monocular decamouflage , 2006, Vision Research.

[39]  Barbara Gillam,et al.  The Stereoscopic Sliver: A Comparison of Duration Thresholds for Fully Stereoscopic and Unmatched Versions , 2007, Perception.