Working memory load impairs the evaluation of behavioral errors in the medial frontal cortex.

Early error monitoring in the medial frontal cortex enables error detection and the evaluation of error significance, which helps prioritize adaptive control. This ability has been assumed to be independent from central capacity, a limited pool of resources assumed to be involved in cognitive control. The present study investigated whether error evaluation depends on central capacity by measuring the error-related negativity (Ne/ERN) in a flanker paradigm while working memory load was varied on two levels. We used a four-choice flanker paradigm in which participants had to classify targets while ignoring flankers. Errors could be due to responding either to the flankers (flanker errors) or to none of the stimulus elements (nonflanker errors). With low load, the Ne/ERN was larger for flanker errors than for nonflanker errors-an effect that has previously been interpreted as reflecting differential significance of these error types. With high load, no such effect of error type on the Ne/ERN was observable. Our findings suggest that working memory load does not impair the generation of an Ne/ERN per se but rather impairs the evaluation of error significance. They demonstrate that error monitoring is composed of capacity-dependent and capacity-independent mechanisms.

[1]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  Effects of Working Memory Load on Visual Selective Attention: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[2]  T. A. Kelley,et al.  Working Memory Load Modulates Distractor Competition in Primary Visual Cortex , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Tom Eichele,et al.  Semi-automatic identification of independent components representing EEG artifact , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[4]  Marco Steinhauser,et al.  Error significance but not error expectancy predicts error-related negativities for different error types , 2016, Behavioural Brain Research SreeTestContent1.

[5]  Carolin Dudschig,et al.  Short Article: Why do we slow down after an error? Mechanisms underlying the effects of posterror slowing , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Working memory demands modulate cognitive control in the Stroop paradigm , 2013, Psychological research.

[7]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Risk-Sensitivity in Sensorimotor Control , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[8]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[9]  D. Ruchkin,et al.  Short-term memory storage and retention: an event-related brain potential study. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[10]  M. Coles,et al.  Dissociation of Pe and ERN/Ne in the conscious recognition of an error. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[11]  Marco Steinhauser,et al.  Enhanced error-related negativity on flanker errors: error expectancy or error significance? , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[12]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[13]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[14]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[15]  Martin E. Maier,et al.  Updating Expected Action Outcome in the Medial Frontal Cortex Involves an Evaluation of Error Type , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Tzyy-Ping Jung,et al.  Independent Component Analysis of Electroencephalographic Data , 1995, NIPS.

[17]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[18]  N. Lavie Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[20]  James F. Cavanagh,et al.  Multiple aspects of the stress response under social evaluative threat: An electrophysiological investigation , 2008, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[21]  R. Kotov,et al.  Beyond the Broken Error-Related Negativity: Functional and Diagnostic Correlates of Error Processing in Psychosis , 2012, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Attentional limitations in dual-task performance. , 1998 .

[23]  B. Burle,et al.  Action Monitoring and Medial Frontal Cortex: Leading Role of Supplementary Motor Area , 2014, Science.

[24]  Tim P. Moran,et al.  On the relationship between anxiety and error monitoring: a meta-analysis and conceptual framework , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[25]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Enhanced detection of artifacts in EEG data using higher-order statistics and independent component analysis , 2007, NeuroImage.

[26]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[27]  Patrick Rabbitt,et al.  Consciousness is slower than you think , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[28]  Thérèse J. M. Overbeek,et al.  Dissociable Components of Error Processing on the Functional Significance of the Pe Vis-à-vis the Ern/ne Performance Monitoring Processes Reflected in the Ne and Pe Review of Studies That Report Both Ne and Pe: Associations and Dissociations Pharmacological Effects , 2022 .

[29]  N. Yeung,et al.  On the ERN and the significance of errors. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[30]  Marco Steinhauser,et al.  Error-related brain activity and adjustments of selective attention following errors , 2011, NeuroImage.

[31]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[32]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[33]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  E. Viding,et al.  Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[35]  Jutta Stahl,et al.  Percept-based and object-based error processing: An experimental dissociation of error-related negativity and error positivity , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[36]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[37]  Avram J Holmes,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of error processing dysfunctions in major depressive disorder. , 2008, Archives of general psychiatry.

[38]  M. Steinhauser,et al.  Isolating Component Processes of Posterror Slowing With the Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[39]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  An Information-Maximization Approach to Blind Separation and Blind Deconvolution , 1995, Neural Computation.

[40]  Tobias U. Hauser,et al.  Conflict monitoring and error processing: New insights from simultaneous EEG–fMRI , 2015, NeuroImage.

[41]  Marco Steinhauser,et al.  Is the Error-related Negativity Amplitude Related to Error Detectability? Evidence from Effects of Different Error Types , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  M. Coles,et al.  Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  J. Grafman,et al.  Distinctions and similarities among working memory processes: an event-related potential study. , 1992, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.