Neural Processes of Proactive and Reactive Controls Modulated by Motor-Skill Experiences

This study investigated the experience of open and closed motor skills on modulating proactive and reactive control processes in task switching. Fifty-four participants who were open-skilled (n = 18) or closed-skilled athletes (n = 18) or non-athletic adults (n = 18) completed a cued task-switching paradigm task. This task tapped into proactive or reactive controls of executive functions under different validity conditions. Electroencephalograms of the participants were captured during the task. In the 100% validity condition, the open-skilled participants showed significantly lower switch cost of response time than the closed-skilled and control participants. Results showed that the open-skilled participants had less positive-going parietal cue-locked P3 in the switch than repeat trials. Participants in the control group showed more positive-going cue-locked P3 in the switch than repeat trials, whereas the closed-skilled participants had no significant differences between the two types of trials. In the 50% validity condition, the open- and closed-skilled participants had less switch cost of response time than the control participants. Participants in the open- and closed-skilled groups showed less positive-going parietal stimulus-locked P3 in the switch than repeat trials, which was not the case for those in the control group. Our findings confirm the dissociation between proactive and reactive controls in relation to their modulations by the different motor-skill experiences. Both proactive and reactive controls of executive functions could be strengthened by exposing individuals to anticipatory or non-anticipatory enriched environments, suggesting proactive and reactive controls involved in motor-skill development seem to be transferable to domain-general executive functions.

[1]  Bolton K. H. Chau,et al.  Motor skill experience modulates executive control for task switching. , 2017, Acta psychologica.

[2]  F. Russo,et al.  Exercise-related cognitive effects on sensory-motor control in athletes and drummers compared to non-athletes and other musicians , 2017, Neuroscience.

[3]  Chun-Hao Wang,et al.  Neural Correlates of Expert Behavior During a Domain-Specific Attentional Cueing Task in Badminton Players. , 2017, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[4]  Cheng-Ta Yang,et al.  Motor expertise modulates neural oscillations and temporal dynamics of cognitive control , 2017, NeuroImage.

[5]  Marika Berchicci,et al.  Different proactive and reactive action control in fencers’ and boxers’ brain , 2017, Neuroscience.

[6]  A. Vallesi,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of the cognitive control processes underpinning mixing and switching costs , 2016, Brain Research.

[7]  Venke Arntsberg Grane,et al.  ERP Correlates of Proactive and Reactive Cognitive Control in Treatment-Naïve Adult ADHD , 2016, PloS one.

[8]  Silvia Conforto,et al.  Proficient brain for optimal performance: the MAP model perspective , 2016, PeerJ.

[9]  A. Fu,et al.  Imagery perspective among young athletes: Differentiation between external and internal visual imagery , 2015, Journal of sport and health science.

[10]  Kosuke Akatsuka,et al.  Skill-Specific Changes in Somatosensory Nogo Potentials in Baseball Players , 2015, PloS one.

[11]  Yuejia Luo,et al.  Enhanced response inhibition in experienced fencers , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[12]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Lifelong physical activity and executive functions in older age assessed by memory based task switching , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Tim Curran,et al.  Metacognitive Processes in Executive Control Development: The Case of Reactive and Proactive Control , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Chia-Liang Tsai,et al.  Exercise-mode-related changes in task-switching performance in the elderly , 2015, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[15]  J. Oosterlaan,et al.  Executive Functioning in Highly Talented Soccer Players , 2014, PloS one.

[16]  R. Knight,et al.  Preparatory attention after lesions to the lateral or orbital prefrontal cortex – An event-related potentials study , 2013, Brain Research.

[17]  Neil G. Muggleton,et al.  Open vs. Closed Skill Sports and the Modulation of Inhibitory Control , 2013, PloS one.

[18]  E. Vuurman,et al.  Higher, faster, stronger: The effect of dynamic stimuli on response preparation and CNV amplitude , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[19]  C. Juan,et al.  Temporal Preparation in Athletes: A Comparison of Tennis Players and Swimmers With Sedentary Controls , 2013, Journal of motor behavior.

[20]  Stephen B Gilbert,et al.  The temporal dynamics of medial and lateral frontal neural activity related to proactive cognitive control , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  Hiroki Nakamoto,et al.  Experts in fast-ball sports reduce anticipation timing cost by developing inhibitory control , 2012, Brain and Cognition.

[22]  M. Wang,et al.  Neural Mechanisms Underlying the Cost of Task Switching: An ERP Study , 2012, PloS one.

[23]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Expert Anticipatory Skill in Striking Sports , 2012, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[24]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Neural correlates of attentional and executive processing in middle-age fencers. , 2012, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[25]  E Macaluso,et al.  Action anticipation beyond the action observation network: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in expert basketball players , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[26]  T. Braver The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  S. Hsieh,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of preparation and implementation for different types of task shifts , 2011, Brain Research.

[28]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Diversity of the P3 in the task-switching paradigm , 2011, Brain Research.

[29]  Alan C.-N. Wong,et al.  Fencing expertise and physical fitness enhance action inhibition , 2011 .

[30]  David W. Eccles,et al.  The effect of manipulating context-specific information on perceptual-cognitive processes during a simulated anticipation task. , 2011, British journal of psychology.

[31]  L. Mo,et al.  Event-related potential effects of superior action anticipation in professional badminton players , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[32]  Toshiyuki Someya,et al.  Reduced thalamic volume observed across different subgroups of autism spectrum disorders , 2010, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[33]  A. Kramer,et al.  Are expert athletes 'expert' in the cognitive laboratory? A meta-analytic review of cognition and sport expertise , 2010 .

[34]  M. Hughes,et al.  The spatial and temporal dynamics of anticipatory preparation and response inhibition in task-switching , 2010, NeuroImage.

[35]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  “Neural efficiency” of experts’ brain during judgment of actions: A high-resolution EEG study in elite and amateur karate athletes , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[36]  Yuji Takeda,et al.  Regular physical activity improves executive function during task switching in young adults. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[37]  Werner Sommer,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for the effect of prior probability on response preparation. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[38]  J. Scisco,et al.  Cardiovascular fitness and executive control during task-switching: an ERP study. , 2008, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[39]  Hiroki Nakamoto,et al.  Effects of stimulus–response compatibility in mediating expert performance in baseball players , 2008, Brain Research.

[40]  S. R. Jackson,et al.  Using advance information in dynamic cognitive control: An ERP study of task-switching , 2006, Brain Research.

[41]  Frini Karayanidis,et al.  ERPs dissociate the effects of switching task sets and task cues , 2006, Brain Research.

[42]  Charles H Hillman,et al.  A cross-sectional examination of age and physical activity on performance and event-related brain potentials in a task switching paradigm. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[43]  Noriyuki Kida,et al.  Intensive baseball practice improves the Go/Nogo reaction time, but not the simple reaction time. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[44]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  Components of Switching Intentional Set , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.