Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of approach- and avoidance-related social information.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D. Wentura,et al. Automatic attention to stimuli signalling chances and dangers: Moderating effects of positive and negative goal and action contexts , 2001 .
[2] D. Wentura. Dissociative affective and associative priming effects in the lexical decision task: yes versus no responses to word targets reveal evaluative judgment tendencies. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[3] D. Wentura,et al. About the impact of automaticity in the minimal group paradigm: evidence from affective priming tasks , 1999 .
[4] S. Chaiken,et al. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- Tin Chen, Bargh / Consequences of Automatic Evaluation Immediate Behavioral Predispositions to Approach or Avoid the Stimulus , 2022 .
[5] Dirk Wentura,et al. Activation and Inhibition of Affective Information: for Negative Priming in the Evaluation Task , 1999 .
[6] A. Greenwald,et al. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[7] Rand R. Wilcox,et al. How many discoveries have been lost by ignoring modern statistical methods , 1998 .
[8] R. Wilcox. Introduction to Robust Estimation and Hypothesis Testing , 1997 .
[9] J. Bargh. The automaticity of everyday life. , 1997 .
[10] K. C. Klauer,et al. List-context effects in evaluative priming. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[11] F. Strack,et al. Influence of overt head movements on memory for valenced words: a case of conceptual-motor compatibility. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[12] T. Dalgleish,et al. The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.
[13] Eliot R. Smith. What do connectionism and social psychology offer each other , 1996 .
[14] John T. Cacioppo,et al. The Influence of Motor Processes on Attitudes Toward Novel Versus Familiar Semantic Stimuli , 1996 .
[15] Wolfgang Sucharowski,et al. Sprache und Kognition , 1996 .
[16] R. Fazio,et al. Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[17] K. Mogg,et al. Selective processing of negative information: effects of clinical anxiety, concurrent depression, and awareness. , 1995, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[18] P. Lang. The emotion probe. Studies of motivation and attention. , 1995, The American psychologist.
[19] M. Kindt,et al. Responding to subliminal threat cues is related to trait anxiety and emotional vulnerability: a successful replication of Macleod and Hagan (1992) , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.
[20] J. Cacioppo,et al. Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. , 1994 .
[21] R. Ulrich,et al. Effects of truncation on reaction time analysis. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[22] J. Bargh. The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. , 1994 .
[23] D. Tucker,et al. Motivating the focus of attention. , 1994 .
[24] F. Pratto. Consciousness and Automatic Evaluation , 1994 .
[25] K. Mogg,et al. Effects of anxiety and awareness on colour-identification latencies for emotional words. , 1993, Behaviour research and therapy.
[26] J. Cacioppo,et al. Rudimentary determinants of attitudes. II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[27] K. Mogg,et al. Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[28] R. Zajonc,et al. Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[29] J. Parker,et al. Alexithymia and the processing of emotional stimuli: An experimental study. , 1993 .
[30] T. K. Srull,et al. Handbook of Social Cognition , 1993 .
[31] R. McNally,et al. Cognitive processing of emotional information in panic disorder. , 1992, Behaviour research and therapy.
[32] G. Peeters,et al. Evaluative meanings of adjectives in vitro and in context : some theoretical implications and practical consequences of positive-negative asymmetry and behavioral-adaptive concepts of evaluation , 1992 .
[33] J. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .
[34] J. Bargh. The ecology of automaticity: toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.
[35] O. John,et al. Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[36] Shelley E. Taylor,et al. Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[37] Scott E. Maxwell,et al. Designing Experiments and Analyzing Data: A Model Comparison Perspective , 1990 .
[38] M. Gurtman,et al. Us and them: Social categorization and the process of intergroup bias. , 1990 .
[39] James L. McClelland,et al. On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.
[40] M. Gurtman,et al. Evidence for the automaticity of ageism , 1990 .
[41] G. Peeters,et al. Positive-Negative Asymmetry in Evaluations: The Distinction Between Affective and Informational Negativity Effects , 1990 .
[42] J. L. Myers,et al. Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[43] P. Lachenbruch. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .
[44] Joseph E LeDoux. Cognitive-Emotional Interactions in the Brain , 1989 .
[45] A. Furnham,et al. The colour naming of emotional words , 1989 .
[46] A. Richards,et al. Colour-identification of differentially valenced words in anxiety , 1989 .
[47] J. Bargh. Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. , 1989 .
[48] K. Mogg,et al. Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states: a replication. , 1989, Behaviour research and therapy.
[49] D. Besner,et al. Word Identification: Imageability, Semantics, and the Content-Functor Distinction , 1988 .
[50] E. Higgins,et al. Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. , 1987, Psychological review.
[51] David E. Kanouse,et al. Negativity in evaluations. , 1987 .
[52] David M. Sanbonmatsu,et al. On the automatic activation of attitudes. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[53] James L. McClelland,et al. Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .
[54] Duncan David Nulty,et al. Construct accessibility, depression and the emotional stroop task: Transient mood or stable structure? , 1986 .
[55] D C Rubin,et al. Predicting which words get recalled: Measures of free recall, availability, goodness, emotionality, and pronunciability for 925 nouns , 1986, Memory & cognition.
[56] Catherine N. Morrison,et al. Frequency of Occurrence of Favorable and Unfavorable Items on the Adjective Check List , 1985 .
[57] W. Hager,et al. Emotionsgehalt Bildhaftigkeit konkretheit und Bedeutungshaltigkeit von 580 Adjektiven: Ein Beitrag zur Normierung und zur Prüfung einiger Zusammenhangshypothesen , 1985 .
[58] C. MacLeod,et al. Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. , 1985, Behaviour research and therapy.
[59] G. Peeters,et al. Relational and informational patterns in social cognition , 1983 .
[60] Serge Moscovici,et al. Current issues in European social psychology , 1983 .
[61] E. Berscheid. The Pollyanna Principle: Selectivity in Language, Memory, and Thought , 1981 .
[62] R. Zajonc. Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .
[63] G. Logan,et al. When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .
[64] W. Kintsch,et al. Memory and cognition , 1977 .
[65] Jacob Cohen,et al. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences , 1979 .
[66] R. Wyer. Changes in meaning and halo effects in personality impression formation. , 1974, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[67] H. Tajfel,et al. Social categorization and intergroup behaviour , 1971 .
[68] Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychologie. Archiv für Psychologie , 1970 .
[69] M. Horwitz,et al. Arousal of ingroup-outgroup bias by a chance win or loss. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[70] C. Osgood,et al. The Pollyanna hypothesis. , 1969 .
[71] Douglas K. Chalmers,et al. Meanings, impressions, and attitudes: A model of the evaluation process. , 1969 .
[72] A. Paivio,et al. Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[73] N. Anderson. PRIMACY EFFECTS IN PERSONALITY IMPRESSION FORMATION USING A GENERALIZED ORDER EFFECT PARADIGM. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[74] Koch Sigmund Ed,et al. Psychology: A Study of A Science , 1962 .
[75] A. K. Solarz,et al. Latency of instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs. , 1960, Journal of experimental psychology.
[76] S. S. Stevens,et al. Handbook of experimental psychology , 1951 .
[77] S. Asch. Forming impressions of personality. , 1946, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[78] N. Lewis. A dynamic theory of personality , 1935 .