Orthographic and phonological coding during visual word matching as related to reading and spelling abilities in college students

College students, scoring average to above-average on WRAT-R subscales for reading and spelling, made match/nonmatch judgments on word pairs either on the basis of phonological similarity (a rhyme match) or orthographic similarity (a visual match). Word pairs rhymed and looked similar, rhymed but looked dissimilar, or looked similar but did not rhyme. Word pairs for which a nonmatch decision had to be made, despite similarity in the irrelevant dimension, produced significantly more errors and longer response latencies. Poorer readers were less vulnerable to phonological interference when making visual discriminations, and poorer readers and spellers were more vulnerable to orthographic interference when making rhyme matches. Results showed that a deficiency in phonological coding and an over-reliance upon orthographic coding, often observed in dyslexic children, can also be seen in relatively poor readers and spellers within a normal adult population.

[1]  J. Rack,et al.  Orthographic and phonetic coding in developmental dyslexia. , 1985, British journal of psychology.

[2]  Maggie Bruck,et al.  The Word Recognition and Spelling of Dyslexic Children. , 1988 .

[3]  Bruce F. Pennington,et al.  Is phonology bypassed in normal or dyslexic development? , 1987, Annals of dyslexia.

[4]  Donald Shankweiler,et al.  The speech code and learning to read. , 1979 .

[5]  E. Boder Developmental Dyslexia: a Diagnostic Approach Based on Three Atypical Reading‐spelling Patterns , 1973 .

[6]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences , 1979 .

[7]  R. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Functions of graphemic and phonemic codes in visual word-recognition , 1974, Memory & cognition.

[8]  P. Bryant,et al.  Independence of Reading and Spelling in Backward and Normal Readers , 1979, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[9]  M. Snowling,et al.  The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. , 1980, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  R Treiman,et al.  Phonological awareness and spelling in normal children and dyslexics: the case of initial consonant clusters. , 1990, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[11]  Foop is still floop: A six year follow-up of phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia , 1990 .

[12]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[13]  F. Manis,et al.  A developmental perspective on dyslexic subtypes , 1988, Annals of dyslexia.

[14]  L. Ehri Learning to Read and Spell Words , 1987 .

[15]  L. Siegel,et al.  Short-term memory processes in children with reading and arithmetic learning disabilities. , 1984 .

[16]  U. Frith Unexpected spelling problems , 1980 .

[17]  P. Bryant,et al.  Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness , 1978, Nature.

[18]  R. Johnston Phonological coding in dyslexic readers. , 1982, British journal of psychology.

[19]  B. Dodd,et al.  The phonological skills of spelling disordered children , 1989 .

[20]  James W. Hall,et al.  Phonemic-similarity effects in good vs. poor readers , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[21]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  When words collide: Orthographic and phonological interference during word processing , 1983, Biological Psychology.

[22]  Bruce F. Pennington,et al.  Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. , 1990, Psychological review.

[23]  Mark D. Jackson,et al.  Further evidence for a relationship between memory access and reading ability , 1980 .

[24]  N. Ellis Visual and name coding in dyslexic children , 1981, Psychological research.

[25]  R. Joshi,et al.  Specific spelling disability: Factual or artifactual? , 1990 .

[26]  Priscilla L. Griffith,et al.  Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. , 1986 .

[27]  Edward L. Thorndike,et al.  A teacher’s word book of the twenty thousand words found most frequently and widely in general reading for children and young people , 1932 .

[28]  E Donchin,et al.  Brain potentials as indices of orthographic and phonological interaction during word matching. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  T. Rasinski,et al.  READING AND SPELLING CONNECTIONS IN THIRD AND FIFTH GRADE STUDENTS , 1989 .

[30]  P. Bryant,et al.  Categorizing sounds and learning to read—a causal connection , 1983, Nature.

[31]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Orthographic effects on rhyme monitoring. , 1979 .

[32]  D. Fulker,et al.  Specific Deficits in Component Reading and Language Skills , 1989, Journal of learning disabilities.