Larger Error Signals in Major Depression are Associated with Better Avoidance Learning

The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is particularly reactive to signals of error, punishment, and conflict in the service of behavioral adaptation and it is consistently implicated in the etiology of major depressive disorder (MDD). This association makes conceptual sense, given that MDD has been associated with hyper-reactivity in neural systems associated with punishment processing. Yet in practice, depression-related variance in measures of mPFC functioning often fails to relate to performance. For example, neuroelectric reflections of mediofrontal error signals are often found to be larger in MDD, but a deficit in post-error performance suggests that these error signals are not being used to rapidly adapt behavior. Thus, it remains unknown if depression-related variance in error signals reflects a meaningful alteration in the use of error or punishment information. However, larger mediofrontal error signals have also been related to another behavioral tendency: increased accuracy in avoidance learning. The integrity of this error-avoidance system remains untested in MDD. In this study, EEG was recorded as 21 symptomatic, drug-free participants with current or past MDD and 24 control participants performed a probabilistic reinforcement learning task. Depressed participants had larger mid-frontal EEG responses to error feedback than controls. The direct relationship between error signal amplitudes and avoidance learning accuracy was replicated. Crucially, this relationship was stronger in depressed participants for high conflict “lose–lose” situations, demonstrating a selective alteration of avoidance learning. This investigation provided evidence that larger error signal amplitudes in depression are associated with increased avoidance learning, identifying a candidate mechanistic model for hypersensitivity to negative outcomes in depression.

[1]  A. Holmes,et al.  Effects of task-relevant incentives on the electrophysiological correlates of error processing in major depressive disorder , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[2]  Lorna C. Quandt,et al.  Error detection and posterror behavior in depressed undergraduates. , 2008, Emotion.

[3]  P. Liddle,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for abnormal error monitoring in recurrent major depressive disorder. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[4]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Genetic triple dissociation reveals multiple roles for dopamine in reinforcement learning , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  Avram J Holmes,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of error processing dysfunctions in major depressive disorder. , 2008, Archives of general psychiatry.

[6]  B. Vogt,et al.  Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Resting anterior cingulate activity and abnormal responses to errors in subjects with elevated depressive symptoms: A 128‐channel EEG study , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[8]  Tim Curran,et al.  Cross-task individual differences in error processing: Neural, electrophysiological, and genetic components , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[9]  D. Tucker,et al.  Frontolimbic response to negative feedback in clinical depression. , 2003, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[10]  W. Hulstijn,et al.  Action monitoring and depressive symptom reduction in major depressive disorder. , 2009, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[11]  T. Robbins,et al.  Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar depression: the influence of perceived failure on subsequent performance , 1996, Psychological Medicine.

[12]  Pearl H Chiu,et al.  Neural evidence for enhanced error detection in major depressive disorder. , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.

[13]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Doreen M. Olvet,et al.  Depression symptom severity and error-related brain activity , 2010, Psychiatry Research.

[15]  Wouter Hulstijn,et al.  Action monitoring in major depressive disorder with psychomotor retardation , 2008, Cortex.

[16]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism , 2004, Science.

[17]  R. Davidson,et al.  Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience. , 2002, Annual review of psychology.

[18]  James F. Cavanagh,et al.  Altered cingulate sub-region activation accounts for task-related dissociation in ERN amplitude as a function of obsessive-compulsive symptoms , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  T. Insel,et al.  Wesleyan University From the SelectedWorks of Charles A . Sanislow , Ph . D . 2010 Research Domain Criteria ( RDoC ) : Toward a New Classification Framework for Research on Mental Disorders , 2018 .

[20]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Dynamic Dopamine Modulation in the Basal Ganglia: A Neurocomputational Account of Cognitive Deficits in Medicated and Nonmedicated Parkinsonism , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Hold Your Horses: Impulsivity, Deep Brain Stimulation, and Medication in Parkinsonism , 2007, Science.

[22]  Avram J Holmes,et al.  Task feedback effects on conflict monitoring and executive control: relationship to subclinical measures of depression. , 2007, Emotion.

[23]  J. Roiser,et al.  Reward and Punishment Processing in Depression , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[24]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Error-Related Brain Activity in Patients with Obsessive- Compulsive Disorder and in Healthy Controls , 2005 .

[25]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Error-Related Negativity Predicts Reinforcement Learning and Conflict Biases , 2005, Neuron.

[26]  B. Vogt Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  T. Robbins,et al.  Approach and avoidance learning in patients with major depression and healthy controls: relation to anhedonia , 2009, Psychological Medicine.

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  Abnormal response to negative feedback in unipolar depression: evidence for a diagnosis specific impairment , 1997, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[29]  B. Jaffe Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders , 1977 .

[30]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[31]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Social stress reactivity alters reward and punishment learning. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[32]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[33]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[34]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[35]  Hauke R. Heekeren,et al.  Temporal dynamics of prediction error processing during reward-based decision making , 2010, NeuroImage.

[36]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[37]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Medial frontal cortex and response conflict: Evidence from human intracranial EEG and medial frontal cortex lesion , 2008, Brain Research.