High Numerates Count Icons and Low Numerates Process Large Areas in Pictographs: Results of an Eye‐Tracking Study

In two experiments, we investigated the influence of numeracy on individuals' information processing of pictographs depending on numeracy via an eye-tracker. In two conditions, participants from the general population were presented with a scenario depicting the risk of having cancer and were asked to indicate their perceived risk. The risk level was high (63%) in experiment 1 (N = 70) and low (6%) in experiment 2 (N = 69). In the default condition, participants were free to use their default strategy for information processing. In the guiding-toward-the-number condition, they were prompted to count icons in the pictograph by answering with an explicit number. We used eye-tracking parameters related to the distance between sequential fixations to analyze participants' strategies for processing numerical information. In the default condition, the higher the numeracy was, the shorter the distances traversed in the pictograph were, indicating that participants counted the icons. People lower in numeracy performed increased large-area processing by comparing highlighted and nonhighlighted parts of the pictograph. In the guiding-toward-the-number condition, participants used short distances regardless of their numeracy, supporting the notion that short distances represent counting. Despite the different default processing strategies, participants processed the pictograph with a similar depth and derived similar risk perceptions. The results show that pictographs are beneficial for communicating medical risk. Pictographs make the gist salient by making the part-to-whole relationship visually available, and they facilitate low numerates' non-numeric processing of numerical information. Contemporaneously, pictographs allow high numerates to numerically process and rely on the number depicted in the pictograph.

[1]  V. Reyna,et al.  Physician decision making and cardiac risk: effects of knowledge, risk perception, risk tolerance, and fuzzy processing. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[2]  J. Chapman,et al.  Principles, Pitfalls, and the Magic of Statistics , 1996 .

[3]  V. Reyna A new intuitionism: Meaning, memory, and development in Fuzzy-Trace Theory. , 2012, Judgment and decision making.

[4]  Michael Schulte-Mecklenbeck,et al.  The role of process data in the development and testing of process models of judgment and decision making , 2011 .

[5]  M. Brundage,et al.  Further Insight into the Perception of Quantitative Information: Judgments of Gist in Treatment Decisions , 2007, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[6]  Valerie F. Reyna,et al.  Semantic coherence and fallacies in estimating joint probabilities. , 2010 .

[7]  I. Lipkus Future Recommendations Numeric , Verbal , and Visual Formats of Conveying Health Risks : Suggested Best Practices and , 2007 .

[8]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Using Visual Aids to Improve Communication of Risks about Health: A Review , 2012, TheScientificWorldJournal.

[9]  J. Sinacore Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions , 1993 .

[10]  V. Reyna,et al.  Risk and Rationality in Adolescent Decision Making , 2006, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[11]  Michael Siegrist,et al.  Numeric and graphic risk information processing of high and low numerates in the intuitive and deliberative decision modes: An eye-tracker study , 2014, Judgment and Decision Making.

[12]  S. Frederick Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 25–42 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making , 2022 .

[13]  Angela Fagerlin,et al.  Presenting Research Risks and Benefits to Parents: Does Format Matter? , 2010, Anesthesia and analgesia.

[14]  Charles J. Brainerd,et al.  The origins of probability judgment: A review of data and theories. , 1994 .

[15]  Valerie F Reyna,et al.  Developmental Reversals in Risky Decision Making , 2014, Psychological science.

[16]  M. Schapira,et al.  The Influence of Graphic Format on Breast Cancer Risk Communication , 2006, Journal of health communication.

[17]  P. Ubel,et al.  Communicating side effect risks in a tamoxifen prophylaxis decision aid: the debiasing influence of pictographs. , 2008, Patient education and counseling.

[18]  James Hyde,et al.  Numeracy and Communication with Patients: They Are Counting on Us , 2008, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[19]  Raj M Ratwani,et al.  Thinking graphically: Connecting vision and cognition during graph comprehension. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[20]  Valerie F Reyna,et al.  Theoretically motivated interventions for reducing sexual risk taking in adolescence: a randomized controlled experiment applying fuzzy-trace theory. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[21]  M. Just,et al.  Eye fixations and cognitive processes , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  Ellen Peters,et al.  Understanding the Role of Numeracy in Health: Proposed Theoretical Framework and Practical Insights , 2009, Health education & behavior : the official publication of the Society for Public Health Education.

[23]  E. Peters Beyond Comprehension , 2012 .

[24]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  The Role of Numeracy in Understanding the Benefit of Screening Mammography , 1997, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[25]  Jessica S. Ancker,et al.  The Practice of Informatics: Design Features of Graphs in Health Risk Communication: A Systematic Review , 2006, J. Am. Medical Informatics Assoc..

[26]  A. Stiggelbout,et al.  Different formats for communicating surgical risks to patients and the effect on choice of treatment. , 2004, Patient education and counseling.

[27]  Valerie F Reyna,et al.  Neurobiological and memory models of risky decision making in adolescents versus young adults. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  V. Reyna Risk perception and communication in vaccination decisions: a fuzzy-trace theory approach. , 2012, Vaccine.

[29]  B. Zikmund‐Fisher,et al.  The Effect of Format on Parents' Understanding of the Risks and Benefits of Clinical Research: A Comparison Between Text, Tables, and Graphics , 2010, Journal of health communication.

[30]  Andreas Glöckner,et al.  An eye‐tracking study on information processing in risky decisions: Evidence for compensatory strategies based on automatic processes , 2011 .

[31]  K. Fujita,et al.  Psychological distance can improve decision making under information overload via gist memory. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[32]  V. Reyna,et al.  How numeracy influences risk comprehension and medical decision making. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  P. Ubel,et al.  The impact of the format of graphical presentation on health-related knowledge and treatment choices. , 2008, Patient education and counseling.

[34]  Laura Chamberlain Eye Tracking Methodology; Theory and Practice , 2007 .

[35]  P. Ubel,et al.  Helping patients decide: ten steps to better risk communication. , 2011, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[36]  Michael Siegrist,et al.  Effect of Risk Ladder Format on Risk Perception in High‐ and Low‐Numerate Individuals , 2009, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[37]  C. K. Mertz,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Numeracy and Decision Making , 2022 .

[38]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Science Current Directions in Psychological , 2010 .

[39]  V. Reyna CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE How People Make Decisions That Involve Risk A Dual-Processes Approach , 2022 .

[40]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Do Icon Arrays Help Reduce Denominator Neglect? , 2010, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[41]  P. Slovic,et al.  Numeracy skill and the communication, comprehension, and use of risk-benefit information. , 2007, Health affairs.

[42]  V. Reyna,et al.  A Theory of Medical Decision Making and Health: Fuzzy Trace Theory , 2008, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[43]  M. Just,et al.  The role of eye-fixation research in cognitive psychology , 1976 .

[44]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Using icon arrays to communicate medical risks: overcoming low numeracy. , 2009, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[45]  Michael Siegrist,et al.  Risk communication with pictographs: The role of numeracy and graph processing , 2011, Judgment and Decision Making.

[46]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Response mode, compatibility, and dual-processes in the evaluation of simple gambles: An eye-tracking investigation , 2012, Judgment and Decision Making.

[47]  B. Rimer,et al.  General Performance on a Numeracy Scale among Highly Educated Samples , 2001, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[48]  P. Ubel,et al.  Reducing the Influence of Anecdotal Reasoning on People’s Health Care Decisions: Is a Picture Worth a Thousand Statistics? , 2005, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[49]  Carmen Keller,et al.  Using a Familiar Risk Comparison Within a Risk Ladder to Improve Risk Understanding by Low Numerates: A Study of Visual Attention , 2011, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[50]  V. Reyna,et al.  Dual Processes in Decision Making and Developmental Neuroscience: A Fuzzy-Trace Model. , 2011, Developmental review : DR.

[51]  P. Ubel,et al.  A Demonstration of ‘‘Less Can Be More’’ in Risk Graphics , 2010, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[52]  V F Reyna,et al.  The Importance of Memory in Informed Consent for Surgical Risk , 2001, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[53]  James P. Kahan,et al.  A PDP-11/45 program for playing n-person characteristic function games , 1976 .

[54]  Ellen Peters,et al.  Numeracy and the Perception and Communication of Risk , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[55]  Angela Fagerlin,et al.  Clinical Implications of Numeracy: Theory and Practice , 2008, Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine.

[56]  Daniel Västfjäll,et al.  Intuitive Numbers Guide Decisions , 2008, Judgment and Decision Making.

[57]  Ellen Peters,et al.  Development and Testing of an Abbreviated Numeracy Scale: A Rasch Analysis Approach: Rasch-Based Numeracy Scale , 2013 .

[58]  M A Just,et al.  A theory of reading: from eye fixations to comprehension. , 1980, Psychological review.

[59]  Andreas Glöckner,et al.  How Distinct are Intuition and Deliberation? An Eye-Tracking Analysis of Instruction-Induced Decision Modes , 2009, Judgment and Decision Making.

[60]  Andreas Glöckner,et al.  The Dynamics of Decision Making in Risky Choice: An Eye-Tracking Analysis , 2012, Front. Psychology.