Functional interaction between entorhinal cortex and basolateral amygdala during trace conditioning of odor aversion in the rat.

In rats, conditioned odor aversion (COA) occurs only if the time interval separating the odor from the subsequent intoxication is very short suggesting that the memory trace of the odor is subject to rapid decay. Recent results from our laboratory have found that lesion of the entorhinal cortex (EC), and activation of the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA) rendered COA tolerant to long interstimulus interval. The present study examined whether the odor memory trace depends on the interaction between the EC and the BLA. Rats lesioned in the EC received infusions of muscimol (a GABA(A) receptor agonist) into the BLA immediately after the odor presentation during acquisition of COA. Injection of muscimol into BLA prevented tolerance of COA to long interstimulus interval induced by EC lesions. This suggests that EC modulates the short-term memory trace of the odor by controlling the GABAergic activity of the BLA during acquisition of COA.

[1]  K. Rusiniak,et al.  Flavor-illness aversions: the peculiar roles of odor and taste in memory for poison. , 1980, Science.

[2]  G. Scala,et al.  Bicuculline Administration into Basolateral Amygdala Facilitates Trace Conditioning of Odor Aversion in the Rat , 1997, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[3]  J. Miller,et al.  Disruption of neophobia, conditioned odor aversion, and conditioned taste aversion in rats with hippocampal lesions. , 1986, Behavioral and neural biology.

[4]  Muscimol infused Into the entorhinal cortex prior to training blocks the involvement of this area in post‐training memory processing , 1993, Behavioural pharmacology.

[5]  R. M. Beckstead Afferent connections of the entorhinal area in the rat as demonstrated by retrograde cell-labeling with horseradish peroxidase , 1978, Brain Research.

[6]  J. Price,et al.  Projections from the amygdaloid complex and adjacent olfactory structures to the entorhinal cortex and to the subiculum in the rat and cat , 1977, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[7]  G. Lynch,et al.  Hippocampus and olfactory discrimination learning: effects of entorhinal cortex lesions on olfactory learning and memory in a successive-cue, go-no-go task. , 1991, Behavioral neuroscience.

[8]  John Garcia,et al.  The Neural Integration of Feeding and Drinking Habits , 1982 .

[9]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Noradrenergic and cholinergic interactions in the amygdala and the modulation of memory storage , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[10]  H. Taukulis Odor aversions produced over long CS-US delays. , 1974, Behavioral biology.

[11]  W. Hankins,et al.  Dissociation of odor and taste in baitshyness. , 1973, Behavioral biology.

[12]  C. Destrade,et al.  Functional dissociation between lateral and medial entorhinal cortex in memory processes in mice , 1983, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  Y. Ben-Ari,et al.  Afferent connections to the amygdaloid complex of the rat and cat. I. Projections from the thalamus , 1979, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[14]  G Lynch,et al.  Studies on retrograde and anterograde amnesia of olfactory memory after denervation of the hippocampus by entorhinal cortex lesions. , 1986, Behavioral and neural biology.

[15]  M. Gallagher,et al.  Taste-potentiated odor aversion learning: role of the amygdaloid basolateral complex and central nucleus. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[16]  K. Coburn,et al.  Potentiation of odor by taste and odor aversions in rats are regulated by cholinergic activity of dorsal hippocampus , 1987, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[17]  J. Veening Subcortical afferents of the amygdaloid complex in the rat: an HRP study , 1978, Neuroscience Letters.

[18]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Potentiation rather than overshadowing in flavor-aversion learning: an analysis in terms of within-compound associations. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[19]  J. Lorden,et al.  Response suppression to odors paired with toxicosis , 1970 .

[20]  C. Destrade,et al.  Late post-learning effect of entorhinal cortex electrical stimulation persists despite destruction of the perforant path , 1984, Brain Research.

[21]  W. Hankins,et al.  Flavor-illness aversions: potentiation of odor by taste in rats. , 1979, Behavioral and neural biology.

[22]  J M Wyss,et al.  An autoradiographic study of the efferent connections of the entorhinal cortex in the rat , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[23]  John Garcia,et al.  Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement , 1966 .

[24]  M. Sarter,et al.  Involvement of the amygdala in learning and memory: a critical review, with emphasis on anatomical relations. , 1985, Behavioral neuroscience.

[25]  M. Nachman,et al.  Learned taste aversions over long delays in rats: The role of learned safety. , 1974 .

[26]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  Further studies of hippocampal representation during odor discrimination learning. , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[27]  D. Finch,et al.  Convergence of projections from the rat hippocampal formation, medial geniculate and basal forebrain onto single amygdaloid neurons: an in vivo extra- and intracellular electrophysiological study , 1992, Brain Research.

[28]  M. Bianchin,et al.  Effect of the infusion of the GABA-A receptor agonist, muscimol, on the role of the entorhinal cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus in memory processes. , 1994, Behavioral and neural biology.

[29]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Olfactory Learning and Memory in the Rat: A “Model System” for Studies of the Neurobiology of Memory , 1992 .

[30]  B. Ferry,et al.  Neuroanatomical and Functional Specificity of the Basolateral Amygdaloid Nucleus in Taste-Potentiated Odor Aversion , 1995, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[31]  John H. Martin Autoradiographic estimation of the extent of reversible inactivation produced by microinjection of lidocaine and muscimol in the rat , 1991, Neuroscience Letters.

[32]  J. Falk Physiology and Behavior. , 1973 .

[33]  M. Bouton,et al.  Potentiation and overshadowing in odor-aversion learning: Role of method of odor presentation, the distal-proximal cue distinction, and the conditionability of odor , 1986 .

[34]  P. Rozin,et al.  "Learned safety" as a mechanism in long-delay taste-aversion learning in rats. , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[35]  M. Kuperstein,et al.  Cue-sampling and goal-approach correlates of hippocampal unit activity in rats performing an odor-discrimination task , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  John Garcia,et al.  Flavor-illness aversions: The role of the amygdala in the acquisition of taste-potentiated odor aversions , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.

[37]  Joseph L. Price,et al.  Beyond the primary olfactory cortex: olfactory-related areas in the neocortex, thalamus and hypothalamus , 1985 .

[38]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Involvement of the amygdaloid complex in neuromodulatory influences on memory storage , 1990, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[39]  G. Paxinos,et al.  The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates , 1983 .

[40]  N. Braveman,et al.  The combined effects of dosage level and interstimulus interval on the formation of one-trial poison-based aversions in rats , 1975 .