Human Substantia Nigra Neurons Encode Unexpected Financial Rewards

The brain's sensitivity to unexpected outcomes plays a fundamental role in an organism's ability to adapt and learn new behaviors. Emerging research suggests that midbrain dopaminergic neurons encode these unexpected outcomes. We used microelectrode recordings during deep brain stimulation surgery to study neuronal activity in the human substantia nigra (SN) while patients with Parkinson's disease engaged in a probabilistic learning task motivated by virtual financial rewards. Based on a model of the participants' expected reward, we divided trial outcomes into expected and unexpected gains and losses. SN neurons exhibited significantly higher firing rates after unexpected gains than unexpected losses. No such differences were observed after expected gains and losses. This result provides critical support for the hypothesized role of the SN in human reinforcement learning.

[1]  E. Vaadia,et al.  Midbrain dopamine neurons encode decisions for future action , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  S. Hyman,et al.  Neural mechanisms of addiction: the role of reward-related learning and memory. , 2006, Annual review of neuroscience.

[3]  K. Doya,et al.  The computational neurobiology of learning and reward , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[4]  A. Graybiel The basal ganglia: learning new tricks and loving it , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[5]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Midbrain Dopamine Neurons Encode a Quantitative Reward Prediction Error Signal , 2005, Neuron.

[6]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism , 2004, Science.

[7]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards , 2004, Science.

[8]  Saori C. Tanaka,et al.  Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  J. Bolam,et al.  Uniform Inhibition of Dopamine Neurons in the Ventral Tegmental Area by Aversive Stimuli , 2004, Science.

[10]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Dopamine Neurons Can Represent Context-Dependent Prediction Error , 2004, Neuron.

[11]  T. Robbins,et al.  l-Dopa medication remediates cognitive inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  W. Schultz Getting Formal with Dopamine and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[13]  M. Horne,et al.  Comparison of the basal ganglia in rats, marmosets, macaques, baboons, and humans: Volume and neuronal number for the output, internal relay, and striatal modulating nuclei , 2002, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[14]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  J. Hollerman,et al.  Dopamine neurons report an error in the temporal prediction of reward during learning , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[17]  Amy Wenzel,et al.  One hundred years of forgetting: A quantitative description of retention , 1996 .

[18]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[19]  P. Dayan,et al.  A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  W. Schultz,et al.  Preferential activation of midbrain dopamine neurons by appetitive rather than aversive stimuli , 1996, Nature.

[21]  W. Schultz,et al.  Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  J. Wixted,et al.  On the Form of Forgetting , 1991 .

[23]  M. Gabriel,et al.  Learning and Computational Neuroscience: Foundations of Adaptive Networks , 1990 .

[24]  W. Schultz Responses of midbrain dopamine neurons to behavioral trigger stimuli in the monkey. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  W. Schultz,et al.  The activity of pars compacta neurons of the monkey substantia nigra in relation to motor activation , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Dynamic Dopamine Modulation in the Basal Ganglia: A Neurocomputational Account of Cognitive Deficits in Medicated and Nonmedicated Parkinsonism , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .