Neural Correlates of Biased Competition in Premotor Cortex

It has been proposed that whenever an animal faces several action choices, their neural representations are processed in parallel in frontoparietal cortex and compete in a manner biased by any factor relevant to the decision. We tested this hypothesis by recording single-unit activity in dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) while a monkey performed two delayed center-out reaching tasks. In the one-target task, a single target was presented and its border style indicated its reward value. The two-target task was the same except two targets were presented and the value of each was varied. During the delay period of the one-target task, directionally tuned PMd activity showed no modulation with value. In contrast, during the two-target task, the same neurons showed strong effects of the value associated with their preferred target, always in relation to the value of the other target. Furthermore, the competition between action choices was strongest when targets were furthest apart. This angular distance effect appeared in neural activity as soon as cells became tuned, while modulation by relative value appeared much later. All of these findings can be reproduced by a computational model which suggests that decisions between actions are made through a biased competition taking place within a sensorimotor map of potential actions.

[1]  Hansjörg Scherberger,et al.  Context-Specific Grasp Movement Representation in the Macaque Anterior Intraparietal Area , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[3]  S. P. Wise,et al.  Primate frontal cortex: effects of stimulus and movement , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[5]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The Cortical Motor System , 2001, Neuron.

[6]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[7]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  E. Miller,et al.  From rule to response: neuronal processes in the premotor and prefrontal cortex. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  Tirin Moore,et al.  Changes in Visual Receptive Fields with Microstimulation of Frontal Cortex , 2006, Neuron.

[10]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Effects of Stimulus-Response Compatibility on Neural Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2003, Neuron.

[11]  A. Allport,et al.  Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action , 1987 .

[12]  M. Roesch,et al.  Neuronal Activity Related to Reward Value and Motivation in Primate Frontal Cortex , 2004, Science.

[13]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[14]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Value Representations in the Primate Striatum during Matching Behavior , 2008, Neuron.

[15]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Similarity Effect and Optimal Control of Multiple-Choice Decision Making , 2008, Neuron.

[16]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Superior colliculus activity related to concurrent processing of saccade goals in a visual search task. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  P. Glimcher The neurobiology of visual-saccadic decision making. , 2003, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  K. Doya,et al.  Representation of Action-Specific Reward Values in the Striatum , 2005, Science.

[19]  W. Newsome,et al.  Representation of an abstract perceptual decision in macaque superior colliculus. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  D. Hansel,et al.  Competition between Feedback Loops Underlies Normal and Pathological Dynamics in the Basal Ganglia , 2022 .

[21]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Modest gaze-related discharge modulation in monkey dorsal premotor cortex during a reaching task performed with free fixation. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Neural correlates of visual and motor decision processes , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[23]  J. Tanji,et al.  Behavioral planning in the prefrontal cortex , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[24]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Neural activity in primary motor and dorsal premotor cortex in reaching tasks with the contralateral versus ipsilateral arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  P. Cisek Integrated Neural Processes for Defining Potential Actions and Deciding between Them: A Computational Model , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  M. A. Basso,et al.  Modulation of Neuronal Activity in Superior Colliculus by Changes in Target Probability , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Visual and Anticipatory Bias in Three Cortical Eye Fields of the Monkey during an Adaptive Decision-Making Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[29]  W. Newsome,et al.  Matching Behavior and the Representation of Value in the Parietal Cortex , 2004, Science.

[30]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  M. Roesch,et al.  Impact of expected reward on neuronal activity in prefrontal cortex, frontal and supplementary eye fields and premotor cortex. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  R. Andersen,et al.  Target Selection Signals for Arm Reaching in the Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Probabilistic reasoning by neurons , 2007, Nature.

[34]  J. Tanji,et al.  Distinctions between dorsal and ventral premotor areas: anatomical connectivity and functional properties , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  C. Ghez,et al.  Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  G. Schöner,et al.  Dynamic Field Theory of Movement Preparation , 2022 .

[37]  S P Tipper,et al.  Action-based mechanisms of attention. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[38]  R J HERRNSTEIN,et al.  Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. , 1961, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[39]  P. Redgrave,et al.  The basal ganglia: a vertebrate solution to the selection problem? , 1999, Neuroscience.

[40]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action , 2005, Neuron.

[41]  J. Duncan EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: Brain mechanisms of attention , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[42]  O. Neumann Visual Attention and Action , 1990 .

[43]  J. Fodor,et al.  The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology , 1984 .

[44]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Activity in Posterior Parietal Cortex Is Correlated with the Relative Subjective Desirability of Action , 2004, Neuron.

[45]  A. Tversky,et al.  The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. , 1981, Science.

[46]  H. Seo,et al.  Lateral Intraparietal Cortex and Reinforcement Learning during a Mixed-Strategy Game , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.