Informational constraints in human precision aiming

Twelve human subjects performed a reciprocal precision aiming task of varying difficulty (index of difficulty=4, 5, or 6) while vision of the ongoing movement was available either continuously or intermittently. In the intermittent conditions, vision of the moving end-effector was available at regular intervals (equal to 100%, 125%, or 150% of the movement durations measured under continuous visibility conditions) for varying amounts of time (75%, 50%, or 25% of the duration of the interval). Movement time (MT) increased with both increasing task difficulty and decreasing availability of visual information. Increases in MT were brought about by the same systematic changes in the kinematic characteristics of movement, whether task difficulty increased or availability of visual information decreased. At higher levels of task difficulty, subjects organized their movements so as to make visual information available at particular instances (at the start and at the end of the aiming movement).

[1]  D Mottet,et al.  Trajectory formation and speed-accuracy trade-off in aiming movements. , 1998, Comptes rendus de l'Academie des sciences. Serie III, Sciences de la vie.

[2]  J. Kelso,et al.  Theoretical concepts and strategies for understanding perceptual-motor skill: from information capacity in closed systems to self-organization in open, nonequilibrium systems. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  W Spijkers,et al.  On-line visual control of aiming movements? , 1995, Acta psychologica.

[4]  Eric L. Amazeen,et al.  Timing the Selection of Information During Rhythmic Catching. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Three-Dimensional Movement Trajectories in Fitts' Task: Implications for Control , 1987 .

[6]  P. Fitts,et al.  INFORMATION CAPACITY OF DISCRETE MOTOR RESPONSES. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  The speed-accuracy trade-off in manual prehension: effects of movement amplitude, object size and object width on kinematic characteristics , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  William H. Warren,et al.  Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  L. Carlton Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  R. Bootsma,et al.  On the information-based regulation of movement: What Wann (1996) may want to consider , 1997 .

[11]  C. I. Howarth,et al.  Movement Control in a Repetitive Motor Task , 1970, Nature.

[12]  R. Bootsma,et al.  The dynamics of human isometric pointing movements under varying accuracy requirements , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[13]  Denis Mottet,et al.  The dynamics of goal-directed rhythmical aiming , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[14]  Y. Guiard On Fitts's and Hooke's laws: simple harmonic movement in upper-limb cyclical aiming. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[15]  P. Beek,et al.  Learning the cascade juggle: a dynamical systems analysis. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  Yves Guiard,et al.  Two-handed performance of a rhythmical fitts task by individuals and dyads. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Claude E. Shannon,et al.  A mathematical theory of communication , 1948, MOCO.

[18]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.