The Structure of Adult Friendship Choices

This paper examines bias toward status-similarity in adult friendships in Detroit and a West German city. Principles of meeting and "mating," by which strangers are converted to acquaintances and acquaintances to friends, are stated. One of these, the proximity principle, claims that the more similar people are, the more likely they will meet and become friends. This principle is tested in matrices of friendship choice for twelve social characteristics. Two statistical measures of bias are used (odds ratio and marginal ratio) and their properties discussed. Compared to a random-choice model, adult friendships show strong bias toward status similarity for all social characteristics. Bias is strongest for "edge" categories of ranked statuses and for "best" friends. The less similar two people are in social characteristics, the less likely they are to be close friends. Demographic characteristics tend to show more bias toward homogeneous choices than other characteristics. These findings are explained and further analyses of adult friendship structure and dynamics are discussed. Urban research has shown that friendship ties among adults are pervasive and active, particularly when compared to neighbor and coworker ties (Axelrod; Bell and Boat; Bell and Force; Jitodai; Tomeh, a, b). These friendships are sources of social contact, intimacy, and assistance. They are considered primary ties, because of their emphasis on face-to-face contact and positive affect (Cooley; K. Davis; Parsons and Shils). Despite friendship's prevalence and assumed importance, little is known about the social structure of adult friendships. Who is chosen? What goods and services are exchanged? How large and heterogeneous are friendship networks? In this paper, one feature of these primary ties is examined: the structure of friendship choice. Most studies of friendship choice involve children or young adults, often college students (Lindzey and Byrne). There are relatively few reports of friendship choice by adults (Barnes; Berkun and Meeland; Curtis; Ellis; Gans, a, b; Greer; Kahl and Davis; Laumann, a, b; Lazarsfeld and *I thank E. 0. Laumann and F. U. Pappi for use of the Detroit and Altneustadt data sets. Laumann was principal investigator of the Detroit study; Laumann and Pappi, of the Altneustadt study.

[1]  George A. Lundberg,et al.  Social Attraction-Patterns in a Village , 1938 .

[2]  H. Richardson Community of Values as a Factor in Friendships of College and Adult Women , 1940 .

[3]  C. Loomis Political and occupational cleavages in a Hanoverian village, Germany : a sociometric study , 1946 .

[4]  L. Festinger Social pressures in informal groups : a study of human factors in housing / by Leon Festinger, Stanley Schachter and Kurt Back , 1950 .

[5]  T. Parsons,et al.  Toward a General Theory of Action , 1952 .

[6]  J. A. Barnes Class and Committees in a Norwegian Island Parish , 1954 .

[7]  J. Davis,et al.  A Comparison of Indexes of Socio-Economic Status , 1955 .

[8]  Wendell Bell,et al.  Urban Neighborhood Types and Participation in Formal Associations , 1956 .

[9]  Morris Axelrod Urban Structure and Social Participation , 1956 .

[10]  S. Greer Urbanism Reconsidered: A Comparative Study of Local Areas in a Metropolis , 1956 .

[11]  Wendell Bell,et al.  Urban Neighborhoods and Informal Social Relations , 1957, American Journal of Sociology.

[12]  Social Stratification and Social Relations: An Empirical Test of the Disjunctiveness of Social Classes , 1957 .

[13]  F. Heider The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .

[14]  Sociometric Effects of Race and of Combat Performance 1 , 1958 .

[15]  Robert F. Winch,et al.  Mate-Selection: A Study of Complementary Needs , 1959 .

[16]  R. Williams Friendship and Social Values in a Suburban Community: An Exploratory Study , 1959 .

[17]  H. Kelley,et al.  The social psychology of groups , 1960 .

[18]  D. Byrne Interpersonal attraction and attitude similarity. , 1961, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[19]  G. C. Homans,et al.  Social Behavior: Its Elementary Forms. , 1975 .

[20]  T. Newcomb The acquaintance process , 1961 .

[21]  Herbert J. Gans,et al.  The Urban Villagers , 1962 .

[22]  Differential Association and the Stratification of the Urban Community , 1963 .

[23]  Ted T. Jitodai Migration and Kinship Contacts , 1963 .

[24]  P. F. Secord,et al.  Interpersonal Congruency, Perceived Similarity, and Friendship , 1964 .

[25]  P. Lazarsfeld,et al.  Friendship as Social process: a substantive and methodological analysis , 1964 .

[26]  A. K. Tomeh Informal Group Participation and Residential Patterns , 1964, American Journal of Sociology.

[27]  Elliot Aronson,et al.  Gain and loss of esteem as determinants of interpersonal attractiveness , 1965 .

[28]  E. Laumann Prestige And Association In An Urban Community , 1966 .

[29]  B N Adams,et al.  Interaction theory and the social network. , 1967, Sociometry.

[30]  O. D. Duncan,et al.  The American Occupational Structure , 1967 .

[31]  I. Rosow Social integration of the aged , 1968 .

[32]  M. W. Riley,et al.  An inventory of research findings , 1968 .

[33]  Leon H. Mayhew Ascription in Modern Societies , 1968 .

[34]  M. W. Riley,et al.  Aging and society , 1968 .

[35]  Frederick Mosteller,et al.  Association and Estimation in Contingency Tables , 1968 .

[36]  Leo A. Goodman,et al.  How to Ransack Social Mobility Tables and Other Kinds of Cross-Classification Tables , 1969, American Journal of Sociology.

[37]  Alan Booth,et al.  Sex and Social Participation , 1972 .

[38]  A. Tyree,et al.  Mobility ratios and association in mobility tables. , 1973, Population studies.

[39]  W. Michelson Man and his urban environment , 1976 .

[40]  D. Treiman Occupational Prestige in Comparative Perspective , 1977 .