Response interference between functional and structural actions linked to the same familiar object
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Duncan. Selective attention and the organization of visual information. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[2] James L. McClelland,et al. On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.
[3] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[4] A. Sirigu,et al. The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.
[5] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Evidence for visuomotor priming effect , 1996, Neuroreport.
[6] U. Castiello. Grasping a fruit: selection for action. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] J. Duncan,et al. Competitive brain activity in visual attention , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[8] S. Tipper,et al. Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .
[9] C. B. Cave. Very Long-Lasting Priming in Picture Naming , 1997 .
[10] R. Ellis,et al. On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] Myrna F. Schwartz,et al. Function and manipulation tool knowledge in apraxia: Knowing ‘what for’ but not ‘how’ , 2000 .
[12] G. Humphreys,et al. Detection by action: neuropsychological evidence for action-defined templates in search , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[13] A. Treisman,et al. Attention, Space, and Action: Studies in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2001 .
[14] L. Buxbaum. Ideomotor Apraxia: a Call to Action , 2001, Neurocase.
[15] L. Buxbaum,et al. Action matters: The role of action plans and object affordances in selection for action , 2002 .
[16] R. Klatzky,et al. Cognitive representations of hand posture in ideomotor apraxia , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[17] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[18] M. Goodale,et al. Naming and grasping common objects: a priming study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] R. Rumiati,et al. The strategic control of multiple routes in imitation of actions. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] Scott H. Johnson-Frey. The neural bases of complex tool use in humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[21] Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al. Neural representations of graspable objects: are tools special? , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[22] Jonathan S. Cant,et al. No evidence for visuomotor priming in a visually guided action task , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[23] M. Santello,et al. Effects of end-goal on hand shaping. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[24] K. Grill-Spector,et al. Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[25] Leonardo Chelazzi,et al. Selecting and ignoring the component features of a visual object: A negative priming paradigm , 2006 .
[26] Y. Rossetti,et al. No double-dissociation between optic ataxia and visual agnosia: Multiple sub-streams for multiple visuo-manual integrations , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[27] L. Buxbaum,et al. Neural substrates of knowledge of hand postures for object grasping and functional object use: Evidence from fMRI , 2006, Brain Research.
[28] J. Duncan. EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: Brain mechanisms of attention , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[29] Leonardo Chelazzi,et al. Selective Attention to Specific Features within Objects: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[30] Robert Ward,et al. Selection for action and selection for awareness: Evidence from hemispatial neglect , 2006, Brain Research.
[31] Kenneth F. Valyear,et al. Human parietal cortex in action , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[32] Murray Grossman,et al. Left Inferior Parietal Representations for Skilled Hand-Object Interactions: Evidence from Stroke and Corticobasal Degeneration , 2007, Cortex.
[33] D. Rosenbaum,et al. Hand path priming in manual obstacle avoidance: evidence that the dorsal stream does not only control visually guided actions in real time. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] Luca Turella,et al. An object for an action, the same object for other actions: effects on hand shaping , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] Patrick Haggard,et al. On-Line Control of Grasping Actions: Object-Specific Motor Facilitation Requires Sustained Visual Input , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[36] George S. Cree,et al. Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words , 2008, Cognition.
[37] A. Kreiter,et al. Feature-based attention and the suppression of non-relevant object features , 2008, Vision Research.
[38] Scott Sinnett,et al. Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in visual object-based selective attention , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[39] Shihui Han,et al. Differentiating spatial and object-based effects on attention: An event-related brain potential study with peripheral cueing , 2008, Brain Research.
[40] Hansjörg Scherberger,et al. Context-Specific Grasp Movement Representation in the Macaque Anterior Intraparietal Area , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[41] Robert W. Kentridge,et al. Object-based attention and visual area LO , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[42] D. Rosenbaum,et al. Hand path priming in manual obstacle avoidance: Rapid decay of dorsal stream information , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[43] Lauren M. Bylsma,et al. Toward an integrated account of object and action selection: A computational analysis and empirical findings from reaching-to-grasp and tool-use , 2009, Neuropsychologia.