Common ravens, Corvus corax, preferentially associate with grey wolves, Canis lupus, as a foraging strategy in winter

One foraging strategy that scavengers can employ to discover unpredictable food sources is to associate directly with predators who inadvertently provide food. The common raven, a well known feeding generalist, is also a prominent scavenger of wolves' kills and is found to be in close association with this predator. We tested the hypothesis that ravens preferentially associate with wolves in winter as a kleptoparasitic foraging strategy. The presence, absence and behaviour of ravens was documented during winter observations of wolves, coyotes, Canis latrans, and elk, Cervus elaphus, as well as the landscape in the absence of these three species. Ravens were found to be in close association with wolves when they were travelling, resting and hunting prey. In comparison, ravens showed no significant association with coyotes, elk or areas on the landscape in the absence of wolves. We also compared ravens' discovery success of wolf-killed and nonwolf-killed carcasses and their behavioural response upon discovery. Ravens found all wolf kills almost immediately and remained at the carcass to feed alongside wolves after the death of the prey. In contrast, ravens were less successful discovering experimentally placed carcasses in the same study region, and did not land or feed despite the availability of fresh, exposed meat. Our results show that ravens' association with wolves is not just an incidental and proximate by-product of the presence of fresh meat. Instead, we show that ravens preferentially associate with wolves in both the presence and absence of food, resulting in the discovery of carcasses and suppression of ravens' innate fear of novel food sources. Through this mode of social foraging, ravens may experience increased foraging efficiency in the use of an otherwise spatially and temporally unpredictable food source.Copyright 2002 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved .

[1]  V. Gelder,et al.  The Serengeti Lion , 1973 .

[2]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Do common ravens yell because they want to attract others? , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[3]  F. Harrington,et al.  Wolves of the World: Perspectives of Behavior, Ecology and Conservation , 1983 .

[4]  B. Heinrich Why Do Ravens Fear Their Food , 1988 .

[5]  Chris Carbone,et al.  Feeding success in African wild dogs : does kleptoparasitism by spotted hyenas influence hunting group size ? , 1997 .

[6]  H A Isack,et al.  Honeyguides and Honey Gatherers: Interspecific Communication in a Symbiotic Relationship , 1989, Science.

[7]  A. C. Bent Life Histories of North American Jays, Crows and Titmice , 1988 .

[8]  Susan M. Cooper,et al.  Optimal hunting group size: the need for lions to defend their kills against loss to spotted hyaenas , 1991 .

[9]  P. Paquet Winter spatial relationships of wolves and coyotes in Riding Mountain National Park, Manitoba , 1991 .

[10]  H. Pöysä Group foraging in patchy environments: the importance of coarse-level local enhancement , 1992 .

[11]  Bernd Heinrich,et al.  Mind of the Raven , 1999 .

[12]  T. Caro Cheetahs of the Serengeti Plains: Group Living in an Asocial Species , 1994 .

[13]  R. Crabtree,et al.  The Ecological Role of Coyotes on Yellowstone's Northern Range , 1999 .

[14]  W. C. Gasaway,et al.  Food acquisition by spotted hyaenas in Etosha National Park, Namibia: predation versus scavenging , 1991 .

[15]  T. Caraco,et al.  Living in groups: is there an optimal group size? , 1984 .

[16]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Raven roosts are mobile information centres , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  H. Jane Brockmann,et al.  Kleptoparasitism in birds , 1979, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  J. Krebs,et al.  Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach , 1978 .

[19]  T. O. Lemke,et al.  Winter range expansion by the northern Yellowstone elk herd , 1998 .

[20]  S. Creel,et al.  Limitation of African Wild Dogs by Competition with Larger Carnivores , 1996 .

[21]  F. Harrington Ravens Attracted to Wolf Howling , 1978 .

[22]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Fear and food recognition in naive Common Ravens , 1995 .

[23]  B. Heinrich Ravens in Winter , 1989 .

[24]  R. Lyman Ott.,et al.  An introduction to statistical methods and data analysis , 1977 .

[25]  Eric M. Gese,et al.  Foraging ecology of coyotes (Canis latrans): the influence of extrinsic factors and a dominance hierarchy , 1996 .

[26]  J. Eisenberg,et al.  Serengeti: Dynamics of an Ecosystem , 1980 .

[27]  C. Fitzgibbon,et al.  Factors influencing the hunting success of an African wild dog pack , 1993, Animal Behaviour.

[28]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Foraging by common ravens in the presence and absence of territory holders: an experimental analysis of social foraging , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[29]  E. R. Turner Social Feeding in Birds , 1964 .

[30]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Winter foraging at carcasses by three sympatric corvids, with emphasis on recruitment by the raven, Corvus corax , 1988, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[31]  C. Slobodchikoff The ecology of social behavior , 1988 .

[32]  L. Mech,et al.  The Wolf: The Ecology and Behavior of an Endangered Species , 1970 .

[33]  S. A. Barnett,et al.  Learning and instinct in animals. , 1957 .

[34]  Hans Kruuk,et al.  The Spotted Hyena: A Study of Predation and Social Behavior , 1972 .

[35]  C. Clark,et al.  Foraging and Flocking Strategies: Information in an Uncertain Environment , 1984, The American Naturalist.

[36]  J. Terborgh Five New World Primates , 1983 .

[37]  L. Mech [Book review] Wolves, Bison and the Dynamics Related to the Peace-Athabasca Delta in Canada's Wood Buffalo National Park, by L. N. Carbyn, S. M. Oosenbrug, and D. W. Anions , 1995 .

[38]  J. Altmann,et al.  Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. , 1974, Behaviour.

[39]  C. Clark,et al.  The evolutionary advantages of group foraging , 1986 .