Control of human arm movements in two dimensions: paths and joint control in avoiding simple linear obstacles

In order to examine path planning and the control of redundant degrees of freedom in the human arm, the movements of the shoulder, elbow and wrist were recorded as subjects moved a pointer to a target and avoided a simple obstacle. With respect to joint control, the results show that the extra degree of freedom provided by the wrist is incorporated into target movements in a systematic manner for both large and small obstacles; it is not used only when there is no geometrical alternative. For the wrist, two strategies are apparent, depending upon the length of the obstacle. Wrist extension predominates for shorter obstacles, while flexion or extension and flexion predominate for longer obstacles. These wrist movements shorten the effective length of the distal segments (lower arm plus hand and pointer) and thus reduce the excursion required at the proximal joints. In part, they correspond to assuming the most comfortable arm configuration at each point in the new path necessitated by the obstacle and can be described by static cost functions. However, wrist extension is also used to move the hand and pointer away from the obstacle as shoulder and elbow movements carry the wrist itself towards the obstacle. Wrist flexion is also used to move the pointer tip rapidly past the obstacle. These components, which cannot be explained by static cost functions alone, confirm for the human arm the hypothesized use of redundant degrees of freedom in obstacle avoidance. With respect to path planning, the results show that the minimum distance between pointer and obstacle remains fairly constant over a large range of obstacle lengths; this relative invariance is interpreted to support the hypothesis that workspace coordinates are important for movement planning. However, minimum distance and several other path parameters do depend significantly on the orientation and location of the movement in the workspace. This inhomogeneity implies that movement planning does not occur exclusively in workspace coordinates; it suggests an influence of joint space criteria. In frontal movements, for example, the systematic decline in the minimum distance with increasing obstacle length is interpreted as a compromise reducing the amount of extra joint movement and the discomfort of arm configurations.

[1]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  J. Foley The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1968 .

[3]  C. I. Howarth,et al.  The Movement of the Hand towards a Target , 1972, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  C. K. Yuen,et al.  Digital Filters , 1979, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[5]  P. Viviani,et al.  Trajectory determines movement dynamics , 1982, Neuroscience.

[6]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Human arm trajectory formation. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  C. D. Gelatt,et al.  Optimization by Simulated Annealing , 1983, Science.

[8]  N. Hogan An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  A. Georgopoulos On reaching. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  N Hogan,et al.  Planning and execution of multijoint movements. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[12]  F. Lacquaniti Central representations of human limb movement as revealed by studies of drawing and handwriting , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[13]  Tamar Flash,et al.  The Organization of Human Arm Trajectory Control , 1990 .

[14]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. 1954. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  M. Kawato,et al.  Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[16]  H. Cruse,et al.  On the cost functions for the control of the human arm movement , 1990, Biological Cybernetics.

[17]  P. Morasso Spatial control of arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Effect of target size on spatial and temporal characteristics of a pointing movement in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  H. Cruse Constraints for joint angle control of the human arm , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[20]  H. Cruse,et al.  The human arm as a redundant manipulator: The control of path and joint angles , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[21]  T. Flash,et al.  The control of hand equilibrium trajectories in multi-joint arm movements , 1987, Biological Cybernetics.

[22]  W. L. Nelson Physical principles for economies of skilled movements , 1983, Biological Cybernetics.