The use of tense and agreement by Hungarian-speaking children with language impairment.

PURPOSE Hungarian is a null-subject language with both agglutinating and fusional elements in its verb inflection system, and agreement between the verb and object as well as between the verb and subject. These characteristics make this language a good test case for alternative accounts of the grammatical deficits of children with language impairment (LI). METHOD Twenty-five children with LI and 25 younger children serving as vocabulary controls (VC) repeated sentences whose verb inflections were masked by a cough. The verb inflections marked distinctions according to tense, person, number, and definiteness of the object. RESULTS The children with LI were significantly less accurate than the VC children but generally showed the same performance profile across the inflection types. For both groups of children, the frequency of occurrence of the inflection in the language was a significant predictor of accuracy level. The two groups of children were also similar in their pattern of errors. Inflections produced in place of the correct inflection usually differed from the correct form on a single dimension (e.g., tense or definiteness), though no single dimension was consistently problematic. CONCLUSIONS Accounts that assume problems specific to agreement do not provide an explanation for the observed pattern of findings. The findings are generally compatible with accounts that assume processing limitations in children with LI, such as the morphological richness account. One nonmorphosyntactic factor (the retention of sequences of sounds) appeared to be functionally related to inflection accuracy and may prove to be important in a language with numerous inflections such as Hungarian.

[1]  R. M. Warren Perceptual Restoration of Missing Speech Sounds , 1970, Science.

[2]  V. Volterra,et al.  Specific language impairment in children: A cross-linguistic study , 1987, Brain and Language.

[3]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Functionalism and the competition model , 1989 .

[4]  M. Rispoli,et al.  The mosaic acquisition of grammatical relations , 1991, Journal of Child Language.

[5]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Working Memory and Language , 2018, Working Memories.

[6]  A D Baddeley,et al.  The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition: a test of phonological working memory. , 1994, Memory.

[7]  Harald Clahsen,et al.  Formal features in impaired grammars: A comparison of English and German SLI children , 1997, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[8]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  Double Agreement: Role Identification in Hungarian , 1997 .

[9]  The Nature of Object Agreement in Hungarian , 1997 .

[10]  J. Zwart The Minimalist Program , 1998, Journal of Linguistics.

[11]  T. Campbell,et al.  Nonword repetition and child language impairment. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[12]  H. Tager-Flusberg,et al.  Present and future possibilities for defining a phenotype for specific language impairment. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[13]  Harald Clahsen,et al.  Tense and agreement in Greek SLI: A case study , 1999 .

[14]  L B Leonard,et al.  Verb agreement morphology in Hebrew-speaking children with specific language impairment. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[15]  L B Leonard,et al.  Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[16]  Lynne E. Hewitt,et al.  Children with Specific Language Impairment , 2002 .

[17]  G. Conti-Ramsden Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language impairment (SLI). , 2003, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[18]  András Kornai,et al.  Creating Open Language Resources for Hungarian , 2004, LREC.

[19]  Dezső Németh,et al.  A verbális munkamemória magyar nyelvű vizsgálóeljárásai , 2005 .

[20]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  The Handbook of East Asian Psycholinguistics: The competition model , 2006 .

[21]  D. Bishop,et al.  Distinct genetic influences on grammar and phonological short‐term memory deficits: evidence from 6‐year‐old twins , 2006, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[22]  H. Clahsen,et al.  Structural and Lexical Case in Child German: Evidence From Language-Impaired and Typically Developing Children , 2006 .

[23]  R. Schwartz,et al.  Effect of sentence length and complexity on working memory performance in Hungarian children with specific language impairment (SLI): A cross-linguistic comparison. , 2006, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[24]  András Kornai,et al.  Web-based frequency dictionaries for medium density languages , 2006 .

[25]  Katharine Graf Estes,et al.  Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: a meta-analysis. , 2007, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[26]  Kristine M. Jensen de López,et al.  Test for Reception of Grammar , 2010 .

[27]  Jonathan M. Campbell,et al.  Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test , 2010 .