On the role of attention for the processing of emotions in speech: sex differences revisited.

In a previous cross-modal priming study [A. Schirmer, A.S. Kotz, A.D. Friederici, Sex differentiates the role of emotional prosody during word processing, Cogn. Brain Res. 14 (2002) 228-233.], we found that women integrated emotional prosody and word valence earlier than men. Both sexes showed a smaller N400 in the event-related potential to emotional words when these words were preceded by a sentence with congruous compared to incongruous emotional prosody. However, women showed this effect with a 200-ms interval between prime sentence and target word whereas men showed the effect with a 750-ms interval. The present study was designed to determine whether these sex differences prevail when attention is directed towards the emotional content of prosody and word meaning. To this end, we presented the same prime sentences and target words as in our previous study. Sentences were spoken with happy or sad prosody and followed by a congruous or incongruous emotional word or pseudoword. The interval between sentence offset and target onset was 200 ms. In addition to performing a lexical decision, participants were asked to decide whether or not a word matched the emotional prosody of the preceding sentence. The combined lexical and congruence judgment failed to reveal differences in emotional-prosodic priming between men and women. Both sexes showed smaller N400 amplitudes to emotionally congruent compared to incongruent words. This suggests that the presence of sex differences in emotional-prosodic priming depends on whether or not participants are instructed to take emotional prosody into account.

[1]  C. H. Hansen,et al.  Finding the face in the crowd: an anger superiority effect. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  Colin M. Brown,et al.  The N400 as a function of the level of processing. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[3]  R. Turner,et al.  Gender, social status, and emotional reliance. , 1999, Journal of health and social behavior.

[4]  K. Heilman,et al.  Understanding emotional prosody activates right hemisphere regions. , 1996, Archives of neurology.

[5]  John A Updegraff,et al.  Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. , 2000, Psychological review.

[6]  Dirk Wentura,et al.  Activation and Inhibition of Affective Information: for Negative Priming in the Evaluation Task , 1999 .

[7]  Stefan Zysset,et al.  Gender differences in the activation of inferior frontal cortex during emotional speech perception , 2004, NeuroImage.

[8]  V. Whiffen,et al.  Are gender differences in depression explained by gender differences in co-morbid anxiety? , 2003, Journal of affective disorders.

[9]  D. Hermans,et al.  Evaluative decision latencies mediated by induced affective states. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[10]  K. Heilman,et al.  The role of the right hemisphere in emotional communication. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  C. Nelson,et al.  Event-related potentials to emotional and neutral stimuli. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[12]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Attitudes to the Right: Evaluative Processing is Associated with Lateralized Late Positive Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1996 .

[13]  Alan Booth,et al.  Gender differences in testosterone and cortisol response to competition , 2005, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[14]  R D Hare,et al.  Semantic and affective processing in psychopaths: an event-related potential (ERP) study. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[15]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[16]  R. Johnson A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[17]  Larry Cahill,et al.  Sex‐Related Influences on the Neurobiology of Emotionally Influenced Memory , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  R. Knight,et al.  Transient interference of right hemispheric function due to automatic emotional processing , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  D. Hermans,et al.  The affective priming effect: Automatic activation of evaluative information in memory. , 1994 .

[20]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[22]  Frank Baeyens,et al.  Odours as affective-processing context for word evaluation: A case of cross-modal affective priming. , 1998 .

[23]  S. Kotz,et al.  ERP Evidence for a Sex-Specific Stroop Effect in Emotional Speech , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  E. Paluck,et al.  The contingent smile: a meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.

[26]  Gary E. Raney,et al.  Effects of Automatic Associative Activation on Explicit and Implicit Memory Tests , 1992 .

[27]  M. Junghöfer,et al.  The selective processing of briefly presented affective pictures: an ERP analysis. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[28]  A. Friederici,et al.  Sex differentiates the role of emotional prosody during word processing. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[29]  A. Ohman,et al.  The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  David M. Sanbonmatsu,et al.  On the automatic activation of attitudes. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[31]  D. Deacon,et al.  Event-related potential indices of semantic priming using masked and unmasked words: evidence that the N400 does not reflect a post-lexical process. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[32]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Sex differences in the neural basis of emotional memories , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  Judith A. Hall Gender Effects in Decoding Nonverbal Cues , 1978 .

[34]  K. Zilles,et al.  Recognition of emotional prosody and verbal components of spoken language: an fMRI study. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[35]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related potential asymmetries during the reading of sentences. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[36]  Ray Johnson For Distinguished Early Career Contribution to Psychophysiology: Award Address, 1985 , 1986 .

[37]  M. Bradley,et al.  Large-scale neural correlates of affective picture processing. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[38]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion and motivation II: sex differences in picture processing. , 2001, Emotion.

[39]  E. Hatfield,et al.  Emotional Contagion , 1995 .

[40]  E. Plante,et al.  Time course of word identification and semantic integration in spoken language. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[41]  M. Bradley,et al.  Brain potentials in affective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[42]  K C Klauer,et al.  Does Sunshine Prime Loyal? Affective Priming in the Naming Task , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[43]  E Altenmüller,et al.  Cortical activation patterns of affective speech processing depend on concurrent demands on the subvocal rehearsal system. A DC-potential study. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion and motivation. , 2007 .

[45]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. , 1977 .