Effects of emotion on prospection during decision-making

In two experiments we examined the role of emotion, specifically worry, anxiety, and mood, on prospection during decision-making. Worry is a particularly relevant emotion to study in the context of prospection because high levels of worry may make individuals more aversive toward the uncertainty associated with the prospect of obtaining future improvements in rewards or states. Thus, high levels of worry might lead to reduced prospection during decision-making and enhance preference for immediate over delayed rewards. In Experiment 1 participants performed a two-choice dynamic decision-making task where they were required to choose between one option (the decreasing option) which provided larger immediate rewards but declines in future states, and another option (the increasing option) which provided smaller immediate rewards but improvements in future states, making it the optimal choice. High levels of worry were associated with poorer performance in the task. Additionally, fits of a sophisticated reinforcement-learning model that incorporated both reward-based and state-based information suggested that individuals reporting high levels of worry gave greater weight to the immediate rewards they would receive on each trial than to the degree to which each action would lead to improvements in their future state. In Experiment 2 we found that high levels of worry were associated with greater delay discounting using a standard delay discounting task. Combined, the results suggest that high levels of worry are associated with reduced prospection during decision-making. We attribute these results to high worriers' aversion toward the greater uncertainty associated with attempting to improve future rewards than to maximize immediate reward. These results have implications for researchers interested in the effects of emotion on cognition, and suggest that emotion strongly affects the focus on temporal outcomes during decision-making.

[1]  A. Mathews,et al.  Restriction of working memory capacity during worry. , 2008, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[2]  J. Maner,et al.  The role of risk avoidance in anxiety. , 2006, Behavior therapy.

[3]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. , 2007, Emotion.

[4]  E. Vaadia,et al.  Midbrain dopamine neurons encode decisions for future action , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  S. Reiss Expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic , 1991 .

[6]  Emotional imagery and the differential diagnosis of anxiety. , 1989, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[7]  A. Markman,et al.  The Curse of Planning: Dissecting Multiple Reinforcement-Learning Systems by Taxing the Central Executive , 2013 .

[8]  G. Pezzulo,et al.  The Value of Foresight: How Prospection Affects Decision-Making , 2011, Front. Neurosci..

[9]  L. Green,et al.  Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. , 2001, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[10]  B. Love,et al.  Short-term gains, long-term pains: How cues about state aid learning in dynamic environments , 2009, Cognition.

[11]  Eldad Yechiam,et al.  Recency gets larger as lesions move from anterior to posterior locations within the ventromedial prefrontal cortex , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[12]  M. Freeston,et al.  Generalized anxiety disorder: a preliminary test of a conceptual model. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[13]  P. Glimcher,et al.  JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2005, 84, 555–579 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) DYNAMIC RESPONSE-BY-RESPONSE MODELS OF MATCHING BEHAVIOR IN RHESUS MONKEYS , 2022 .

[14]  K. Buhr,et al.  Investigating the construct validity of intolerance of uncertainty and its unique relationship with worry. , 2006, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[15]  P. Dayan,et al.  States versus Rewards: Dissociable Neural Prediction Error Signals Underlying Model-Based and Model-Free Reinforcement Learning , 2010, Neuron.

[16]  P. Dayan,et al.  Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors , 2011, Neuron.

[17]  W. Ray,et al.  Future-oriented decision-making in Generalized Anxiety Disorder is evident across different versions of the Iowa Gambling Task. , 2010, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[18]  A. MacLeod,et al.  Anxiety, depression, and the anticipation of future positive and negative experiences. , 1996, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[19]  Darrell A. Worthy,et al.  Age-Based Differences in Strategy Use in Choice Tasks , 2012, Front. Neurosci..

[20]  Robert Ladouceur,et al.  Why do people worry , 1994 .

[21]  Kathy D. Gerlach,et al.  Future planning: default network activity couples with frontoparietal control network and reward-processing regions during process and outcome simulations. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[22]  R. Noyes,et al.  Generalized anxiety disorder. , 1987, Primary care.

[23]  J. Lerner,et al.  Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice , 2000 .

[24]  K. Buhr,et al.  The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale: psychometric properties of the English version. , 2002, Behaviour research and therapy.

[25]  Alex Guindon,et al.  Positive mood effects on delay discounting. , 2010, Emotion.

[26]  Gregory T. Smith,et al.  Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: positive and negative urgency. , 2008, Psychological bulletin.

[27]  Gregory L. Stuart,et al.  Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[28]  L. Kvavilashvili,et al.  The predictors of thought suppression in young and old adults: Effects of rumination, anxiety, and other variables. , 2007 .

[29]  Arthur B Markman,et al.  Navigating through abstract decision spaces: Evaluating the role of state generalization in a dynamic decision-making task , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  Jan Peters,et al.  The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  A. Markman,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : General Retrospective Revaluation in Sequential Decision Making : A Tale of Two Systems , 2012 .

[32]  Darrell A. Worthy,et al.  Working-memory load and temporal myopia in dynamic decision making. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  Kaileigh A. Byrne,et al.  Decomposing the roles of perseveration and expected value representation in models of the Iowa gambling task , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[34]  Greg Hajcak,et al.  Intolerance of uncertainty and decisions about delayed, probabilistic rewards. , 2011, Behavior therapy.

[35]  B. Barnaby Clinical Handbook of Psychological Disorders: A Step-by-Step Treatment Manual , 2015 .

[36]  M. Hersen,et al.  Psychometric properties and diagnostic utility of the Beck Anxiety Inventory and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory with older adult psychiatric outpatients. , 1997, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[37]  R. Ladouceur,et al.  Behavioral Expressions of Intolerance of Uncertainty in Worry , 1997, Behavior modification.

[38]  C. Lejuez,et al.  Dispositional anxiety and risk-avoidant decision-making. , 2007 .

[39]  Kaileigh A. Byrne,et al.  Do narcissists make better decisions? An investigation of narcissism and dynamic decision-making performance , 2013 .

[40]  R. Hertwig,et al.  Decisions from Experience and the Effect of Rare Events in Risky Choice , 2004, Psychological science.

[41]  N. Srinivasan,et al.  Role of affect in decision making. , 2013, Progress in brain research.

[42]  J. Beck,et al.  Worry Versus Anxiety , 1998, Behavior modification.

[43]  T. Pruzinsky,et al.  Preliminary exploration of worry: some characteristics and processes. , 1983, Behaviour research and therapy.

[44]  T J Meyer,et al.  Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. , 1990, Behaviour research and therapy.

[45]  V. Starcevic,et al.  Preliminary validation of an ultra-brief version of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. , 2011, Clinical psychology & psychotherapy.

[46]  H. de Wit,et al.  Delay or probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: effect of alcohol. , 1999, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[47]  Darrell A. Worthy,et al.  With Age Comes Wisdom , 2011, Psychological science.

[48]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Neural Dissociation of Delay and Uncertainty in Intertemporal Choice , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[50]  Joseph G. Johnson,et al.  Decision making under risk and uncertainty. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[51]  Arthur B Markman,et al.  Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav , 2012 .

[52]  W. Ray,et al.  Worry: A Cognitive Phenomenon Intimately Linked to Affective, Physiological, and Interpersonal Behavioral Processes , 1998, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[53]  H. Akaike A new look at the statistical model identification , 1974 .

[54]  A. Beck,et al.  An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. , 1988, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[55]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[56]  Robert Ladouceur,et al.  Intolerance of Uncertainty and Problem Orientation in Worry , 1997, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[57]  D. Chambless,et al.  Reliability and validity of the beck anxiety inventory , 1992 .

[58]  G. Dreisbach,et al.  How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[59]  Michel Tuan Pham,et al.  All Negative Moods Are Not Equal: Motivational Influences of Anxiety and Sadness on Decision Making. , 1999, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[60]  R. Hertwig,et al.  The description–experience gap in risky choice , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[61]  Eldad Yechiam,et al.  Comparison of basic assumptions embedded in learning models for experience-based decision making , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[62]  N. Koerner,et al.  A preliminary investigation of impulsivity in generalized anxiety disorder , 2013 .

[63]  Richard L. Metzger,et al.  Worry changes decision making: the effect of negative thoughts on cognitive processing. , 1990, Journal of clinical psychology.

[64]  M. Eysenck,et al.  New perspectives in attentional control theory , 2011 .

[65]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[66]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. , 1994, Annual review of psychology.

[67]  Lao Juan,et al.  Development and Validation of a Scale for Measuring Instructors' Attitudes toward Concept-Based or Reform-Oriented Teaching of Introductory Statistics in the Health and Behavioral Sciences , 2010, 1007.3219.

[68]  Timothy A. Brown,et al.  Generalized Anxiety Disorder , 2015 .

[69]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Prospection: Experiencing the Future , 2007, Science.

[70]  N. Schmidt,et al.  The role of anxiety sensitivity in the pathogenesis of panic: prospective evaluation of spontaneous panic attacks during acute stress. , 1997, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[71]  Geert Vervaeke,et al.  The Penn State Worry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: structure, reliability and validity , 1999 .

[72]  Alice Y. Chiang,et al.  Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[73]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  Of goals and habits: age-related and individual differences in goal-directed decision-making , 2013, Front. Neurosci..