Current understanding of eye guidance

Visual sampling of the scene is at a premium in human vision. High acuity vision is restricted to the small foveal region at the centre of vision and the ‘‘saccade and fixate’’ strategy means that this window of high-resolution vision is only directed at an average of three to four locations in each second. It is therefore not surprising that understanding the factors that underlie decisions about where to allocate this valuable resource of foveal vision have been at the heart of eye movement research since the earliest objective recordings (Wade & Tatler, 2005). The earliest work on viewing complex scenes found that there were certain locations in scenes that were consistently looked at by most observers (Buswell, 1935). Buswell called these locations ‘‘centers of interest’’ and asked what it was about these locations that made them ‘‘interesting’’. He considered that there were two possibilities: That it was something external in the stimulus that attracted the viewers’ eyes; or that it was something more internal to the viewer that reflected higher level cognitive ‘‘interest’’. Buswell favoured the latter explanation, and he famously showed that presenting the same image but with different instructions fundamentally changed the places that a viewer fixated; an observation that was confirmed by Yarbus (1967). Given this demonstration and early favouring of top-down control of where we place our eyes in complex scenes, it is perhaps surprising that much of the subsequent consideration of how we view complex scenes has focused on the role of low-level factors in guiding attention. Why might this be? A wealth of literature on visual search demonstrated that basic visual features

[1]  C. Koch,et al.  A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[2]  C. Koch,et al.  Task-demands can immediately reverse the effects of sensory-driven saliency in complex visual stimuli. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[3]  Michael L. Mack,et al.  VISUAL SALIENCY DOES NOT ACCOUNT FOR EYE MOVEMENTS DURING VISUAL SEARCH IN REAL-WORLD SCENES , 2007 .

[4]  Derrick J. Parkhurst,et al.  Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention , 2002, Vision Research.

[5]  J. Henderson,et al.  Accurate visual memory for previously attended objects in natural scenes , 2002 .

[6]  A. L. Yarbus,et al.  Eye Movements and Vision , 1967, Springer US.

[7]  B. Tatler,et al.  The Moving Tablet Of The Eye , 2005 .

[8]  B. Tatler,et al.  The prominence of behavioural biases in eye guidance , 2009 .

[9]  Antonio Torralba,et al.  Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: the role of global features in object search. , 2006, Psychological review.

[10]  U. Neisser VISUAL SEARCH. , 1964, Scientific American.

[11]  J. Henderson,et al.  How are eye fixation durations controlled during scene viewing? Further evidence from a scene onset delay paradigm , 2009 .

[12]  P. König,et al.  Gaze allocation in natural stimuli: Comparing free exploration to head-fixed viewing conditions , 2009 .

[13]  A. L. I︠A︡rbus Eye Movements and Vision , 1967 .

[14]  R. Baddeley,et al.  Do we look at lights? Using mixture modelling to distinguish between low- and high-level factors in natural image viewing , 2009 .

[15]  L. Chalupa,et al.  The visual neurosciences , 2004 .

[16]  G. Zelinsky,et al.  An effect of referential scene constraint on search implies scene segmentation , 2009 .

[17]  Krista A. Ehinger,et al.  Modelling search for people in 900 scenes: A combined source model of eye guidance , 2009 .

[18]  K. Jellinger The Moving Tablet of the Eye: The Origins of Modern Eye Movement Research , 2006 .

[19]  G. Hauske,et al.  Object and scene analysis by saccadic eye-movements: an investigation with higher-order statistics. , 2000, Spatial vision.

[20]  Miguel P. Eckstein,et al.  Gaze control and memory for objects while walking in a real world environment , 2009 .

[21]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[22]  D. Ringach,et al.  Topological analysis of population activity in visual cortex. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[23]  D. Ballard,et al.  Eye movements in natural behavior , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  B. Tatler,et al.  Looking and Acting: Vision and eye movements in natural behaviour , 2009 .

[25]  Iain D. Gilchrist,et al.  Visual correlates of fixation selection: effects of scale and time , 2005, Vision Research.

[26]  R. C. Langford How People Look at Pictures, A Study of the Psychology of Perception in Art. , 1936 .

[27]  P Reinagel,et al.  Natural scene statistics at the centre of gaze. , 1999, Network.

[28]  Matthew H Tong,et al.  SUN: Top-down saliency using natural statistics , 2009, Visual cognition.

[29]  Benjamin W Tatler,et al.  The central fixation bias in scene viewing: selecting an optimal viewing position independently of motor biases and image feature distributions. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[30]  D. Ballard,et al.  Modelling the role of task in the control of gaze , 2009, Visual cognition.

[31]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  The statistics of natural hand movements , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  M. Land Eye movements and the control of actions in everyday life , 2006, Progress in Retinal and Eye Research.

[33]  Dana H. Ballard,et al.  Modeling embodied visual behaviors , 2007, TAP.

[34]  R. Baddeley,et al.  The long and the short of it: Spatial statistics at fixation vary with saccade amplitude and task , 2006, Vision Research.

[35]  D. S. Wooding,et al.  Fixation sequences made during visual examination of briefly presented 2D images. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[36]  Mary Hayhoe,et al.  Visual Cognition , 2021, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.

[37]  Mary M Hayhoe,et al.  Task and context determine where you look. , 2016, Journal of vision.

[38]  P. König,et al.  Does luminance‐contrast contribute to a saliency map for overt visual attention? , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[39]  Peter König,et al.  Human eye-head co-ordination in natural exploration , 2007, Network.

[40]  V. Bruce,et al.  Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  T. Foulsham,et al.  What can saliency models predict about eye movements? Spatial and sequential aspects of fixations during encoding and recognition. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[42]  B. Velichkovsky,et al.  Distractor effect and saccade amplitudes: Further evidence on different modes of processing in free exploration of visual images , 2009 .

[43]  T. Foulsham,et al.  Saliency and scan patterns in the inspection of real-world scenes: Eye movements during encoding and recognition , 2009 .

[44]  J. Henderson,et al.  Facilitation of return during scene viewing , 2009 .

[45]  B. Velichkovsky,et al.  Two Visual Systems and Their Eye Movements: Evidence from Static and Dynamic Scene Perception , 2005 .

[46]  Christof Koch,et al.  A Model of Saliency-Based Visual Attention for Rapid Scene Analysis , 2009 .

[47]  Roland J. Baddeley,et al.  The nature of the visual representations involved in eye movements when walking down the street , 2009 .

[48]  P. Perona,et al.  Objects predict fixations better than early saliency. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[49]  B. Velichkovsky,et al.  Time course of information processing during scene perception: The relationship between saccade amplitude and fixation duration , 2005 .

[50]  J. Henderson,et al.  Overt attentional prioritization of new objects and feature changes during real-world scene viewing , 2009 .

[51]  A. Kingstone,et al.  The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze , 1998 .

[52]  A. Kingstone,et al.  Get real! Resolving the debate about equivalent social stimuli , 2009 .

[53]  Graham L. Pierce,et al.  Eye movements during scene viewing: Evidence for mixed control of fixation durations , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[54]  F. Hamker,et al.  About the influence of post-saccadic mechanisms for visual stability on peri-saccadic compression of object location. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[55]  G. Kuhn,et al.  You look where I look! Effect of gaze cues on overt and covert attention in misdirection , 2009 .

[56]  Marianne A. DeAngelus,et al.  Top-down control of eye movements: Yarbus revisited , 2009 .

[57]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Visual Attention and the Semantics of Space , 2006, Psychological science.