Psychophysical evidence for the number sense
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Eckart Zimmermann,et al. Numerosity perception after size adaptation , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[2] M. Landy,et al. The effect of viewpoint on perceived visual roughness. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[3] David C Burr,et al. Visual sustained attention and numerosity sensitivity correlate with math achievement in children. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[4] C Blakemore,et al. On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images , 1969, The Journal of physiology.
[5] James T Todd,et al. Are discrimination thresholds a valid measure of variance for judgments of slant from texture? , 2010, Journal of vision.
[6] S. Dumoulin,et al. Topographic representations of object size and relationships with numerosity reveal generalized quantity processing in human parietal cortex , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[7] D. Burr,et al. A Visual Sense of Number , 2007, Current Biology.
[8] Jun Zhang,et al. Connectedness affects dot numerosity judgment: Implications for configural processing , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[9] J. Tanji,et al. Numerical representation for action in the parietal cortex of the monkey , 2002, Nature.
[10] Scott W. Brown. Time, change, and motion: The effects of stimulus movement on temporal perception , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] D. C. Burr,et al. Adaptation to number operates on perceived rather than physical numerosity , 2016, Cognition.
[12] Guido Marco Cicchini,et al. Spontaneous perception of numerosity in humans , 2016, Nature Communications.
[13] David C. Burr,et al. Connecting Visual Objects Reduces Perceived Numerosity and Density for Sparse but not Dense Patterns , 2017, J. Numer. Cogn..
[14] A. Nieder. The neuronal code for number , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[15] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[16] David C. Burr,et al. Separate Mechanisms for Perception of Numerosity and Density , 2014, Psychological science.
[17] J. Ross. Visual Discrimination of Number without Counting , 2003, Perception.
[18] Frank Bremmer,et al. Saccadic Compression of Symbolic Numerical Magnitude , 2012, PloS one.
[19] D. Levi. Crowding—An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review , 2008, Vision Research.
[20] David Burr,et al. Spatiotopic perceptual maps in humans: evidence from motion adaptation , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[21] Albert Yonas,et al. Infants and adults use line junction information to perceive 3D shape. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[22] M. Morgan. A bias-free measure of retinotopic tilt adaptation. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[23] Justin Halberda,et al. Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.
[24] David C. Burr,et al. The effects of cross-sensory attentional demand on subitizing and on mapping number onto space , 2012, Vision Research.
[25] Pierre Pica,et al. Education Enhances the Acuity of the Nonverbal Approximate Number System , 2013, Psychological science.
[26] M. Hawken,et al. Perceived velocity of luminance, chromatic and non-fourier stimuli: Influence of contrast and temporal frequency , 1996, Vision Research.
[27] Ryota Kanai,et al. Time dilation in dynamic visual display. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[28] John Ross,et al. Visual processing of motion , 1986, Trends in Neurosciences.
[29] David C Burr,et al. Adaptation affects both high and low (subitized) numbers under conditions of high attentional load. , 2011, Seeing and perceiving.
[30] Peter Földiák,et al. Adaptation and decorrelation in the cortex , 1989 .
[31] Tiangang Zhou,et al. Topology-defined units in numerosity perception , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[32] Manuela Piazza,et al. Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[33] G. Rhodes,et al. Fitting the Mind to the World: Adaptation and after-effects in high-level vision , 2005 .
[34] David C Burr,et al. Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional resources. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[35] Steven C Dakin,et al. Number and density discrimination rely on a common metric: Similar psychophysical effects of size, contrast, and divided attention. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[36] Vincent Walsh,et al. From magnitude to natural numbers: A developmental neurocognitive perspective , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[37] Guido Marco Cicchini,et al. Mechanisms for perception of numerosity or texture-density are governed by crowding-like effects. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[38] Bert Reynvoet,et al. The Role of Visual Information in Numerosity Estimation , 2012, PloS one.
[39] M Concetta Morrone,et al. Neural mechanisms for timing visual events are spatially selective in real-world coordinates , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[40] P. Mamassian,et al. Predictive Properties of Visual Adaptation , 2012, Current Biology.
[41] Vincent Walsh. A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[42] G. Alvarez,et al. Number estimation relies on a set of segmented objects , 2009, Cognition.
[43] D. Burr,et al. Motion psychophysics: 1985–2010 , 2011, Vision Research.
[44] Anna C Nobre,et al. Tracking the changing feature of a moving object. , 2016, Journal of vision.
[45] F. Kingdom,et al. A common visual metric for approximate number and density , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[46] Brian Butterworth,et al. Are Subitizing and Counting Implemented as Separate or Functionally Overlapping Processes? , 2002, NeuroImage.
[47] D. Burr,et al. A shared numerical representation for action and perception , 2016, eLife.
[48] G. Mandler,et al. Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[49] Steven C. Dakin,et al. Sensitivity to numerosity is not a unique visuospatial psychophysical predictor of mathematical ability , 2013, Vision Research.
[50] Serge O. Dumoulin,et al. Can responses to basic non-numerical visual features explain neural numerosity responses? , 2017, NeuroImage.
[51] S. Solomon,et al. Moving Sensory Adaptation beyond Suppressive Effects in Single Neurons , 2014, Current Biology.
[52] Steven C. Dakin,et al. A texture-processing model of the ‘visual sense of number’ , 2014, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[53] Guido Marco Cicchini,et al. Number As a Primary Perceptual Attribute: A Review , 2016, Perception.
[54] Helen M. Ditz,et al. Neurons selective to the number of visual items in the corvid songbird endbrain , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[55] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans’ mental number line , 2015, Science.
[56] F. Durgin. Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited , 2008, Current Biology.
[57] Sophie M. Wuerger,et al. Input of long- and middle-wavelength-sensitive cones to orientation discrimination , 1999 .
[58] H. Barlow,et al. Evidence for a Physiological Explanation of the Waterfall Phenomenon and Figural After-effects , 1963, Nature.
[59] A. Kohn. Visual adaptation: physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[60] Stephen F. Goodwin,et al. Sexual Dimorphism: Can You Smell the Difference? , 2008, Current Biology.
[61] S. Dehaene,et al. The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .
[62] D. Burr,et al. Vision senses number directly. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[63] Qixuan Chen,et al. Association between individual differences in non-symbolic number acuity and math performance: a meta-analysis. , 2014, Acta psychologica.
[64] S. Dehaene,et al. Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps , 2007, Neuron.
[65] Pooja Viswanathan,et al. Neuronal correlates of a visual “sense of number” in primate parietal and prefrontal cortices , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[66] G. Boynton. Adaptation and attentional selection , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[67] P. Thompson. Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast , 1982, Vision Research.
[68] Frank H. Durgin,et al. Texture density adaptation and the perceived numerosity and distribution of texture. , 1995 .
[69] Ikuya Murakami,et al. Perceived duration of visual motion increases with speed. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[70] A. Henik,et al. The contribution of fish studies to the “number sense” debate , 2016, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[71] M. Morrone,et al. Blood Oxygen Level-Dependent Activation of the Primary Visual Cortex Predicts Size Adaptation Illusion , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[72] D. Burr,et al. Saccades compress space, time and number , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[73] C. Gallistel,et al. Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .
[74] Edward F. Ester,et al. Substitution and pooling in visual crowding induced by similar and dissimilar distractors. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[75] D. Burr,et al. Spatiotopic coding during dynamic head tilt. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.
[76] M. Carandini,et al. Adaptation maintains population homeostasis in primary visual cortex , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.
[77] Elisa Castaldi,et al. Numerosity but not texture-density discrimination correlates with math ability in children. , 2016, Developmental psychology.
[78] B. P. Klein,et al. Topographic Representation of Numerosity in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2013, Science.
[79] Roberto Arrighi,et al. Spatial but Not Temporal Numerosity Thresholds Correlate With Formal Math Skills in Children , 2017, Developmental psychology.
[80] S. Dehaene,et al. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[81] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Does Subitizing Reflect Numerical Estimation? , 2022 .
[82] David C. Burr,et al. Effects of adaptation on numerosity decoding in the human brain , 2016, NeuroImage.
[83] E. Spelke,et al. Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. , 1999, Science.
[84] Sheng He,et al. Larger stimuli are judged to last longer. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[85] J. Tanji,et al. Deficits in action selection based on numerical information after inactivation of the posterior parietal cortex in monkeys. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[86] Michael J. Morgan. A bias-free measure of retinotopic tilt adaptation , 2014 .
[87] D. L. Macadam. Visual Sensitivities to Color Differences in Daylight , 1942 .
[88] M. Srinivasan,et al. Evidence for counting in insects , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[89] D. Burr,et al. Visual aftereffects , 2009, Current Biology.
[90] David C. Burr,et al. A generalized sense of number , 2014, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[91] Christian Agrillo,et al. Individual differences in non-symbolic numerical abilities predict mathematical achievements but contradict ATOM , 2013, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[92] M. Morgan,et al. The computation of relative numerosity, size and density , 2016, Vision Research.
[93] Maria Elena Miletto Petrazzini,et al. Numerical abilities in fish: A methodological review , 2017, Behavioural Processes.
[94] D. Melcher,et al. Subitizing reflects visuo-spatial object individuation capacity , 2011, Cognition.
[95] David Aagten-Murphy,et al. Adaptation to numerosity requires only brief exposures, and is determined by number of events, not exposure duration , 2016, Journal of vision.
[96] Andreas Nieder,et al. Temporal and Spatial Enumeration Processes in the Primate Parietal Cortex , 2006, Science.
[97] J. Ross,et al. Contrast adaptation and contrast masking in human vision , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[98] E. L. Kaufman,et al. The discrimination of visual number. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.
[99] E. Spelke,et al. Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.