Neural correlates of observing joint actions with shared intentions
暂无分享,去创建一个
Shirley-Ann Rueschemeyer | Floris P. de Lange | Terry Eskenazi | Günther Knoblich | Natalie Sebanz | N. Sebanz | G. Knoblich | Shirley-Ann Rueschemeyer | T. Eskenazi | F. P. Lange
[1] C. Chiu,et al. Birds of a feather and birds flocking together: physical versus behavioral cues may lead to trait- versus goal-based group perception. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[2] Roel M. Willems,et al. Complementary Systems for Understanding Action Intentions , 2008, Current Biology.
[3] J. Braithwaite,et al. Seeing it their way: evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] G. Juckel,et al. Cooperation and Deception Recruit Different Subsets of the Theory-of-Mind Network , 2008, PloS one.
[5] Henrik Walter,et al. Understanding Intentions in Social Interaction: The Role of the Anterior Paracingulate Cortex , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[6] Hanneke E. M. den Ouden,et al. Thinking about intentions , 2005, NeuroImage.
[7] C. Macrae,et al. The rhythm of rapport: Interpersonal synchrony and social perception , 2009 .
[8] C. Frith,et al. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[9] Michael E. Bratman,et al. Shared Cooperative Activity , 1991 .
[10] E. D. Paolo,et al. Can social interaction constitute social cognition? , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[11] R. E Passingham,et al. Inferring false beliefs from the actions of oneself and others: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.
[12] N. Andreasen,et al. Visualizing how one brain understands another: a PET study of theory of mind. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.
[13] A. Friederici,et al. Time Perception and Motor Timing: A Common Cortical and Subcortical Basis Revealed by fMRI , 2000, NeuroImage.
[14] Matthew F S Rushworth,et al. The Computation of Social Behavior , 2009, Science.
[15] Hein T. van Schie,et al. Neural mechanisms underlying immediate and final action goals in object use reflected by slow wave brain potentials , 2007, Brain Research.
[16] Nadim Joni Shah,et al. Neural Representations of Self versus Other: Visual-Spatial Perspective Taking and Agency in a Virtual Ball-tossing Game , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[17] Nadim Joni Shah,et al. Duration matters: Dissociating neural correlates of detection and evaluation of social gaze , 2009, NeuroImage.
[18] Christopher D. Frith,et al. Imaging the Intentional Stance in a Competitive Game , 2002, NeuroImage.
[19] Chris Moore,et al. Young children overimitate in third-party contexts. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[20] N. Sadato,et al. Processing of Social and Monetary Rewards in the Human Striatum , 2008, Neuron.
[21] M. Tomasello,et al. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[22] Vince D. Calhoun,et al. Brain Activity Dissociates Mentalization from Motivation During an Interpersonal Competitive Game , 2009, Brain Imaging and Behavior.
[23] Hannes Rakoczy,et al. Minds, persons, and space: An fMRI investigation into the relational complexity of higher-order intentionality , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[24] C. Frith,et al. Rapid Response Subject Collections , 2022 .
[25] Philip R. Cohen,et al. Collective Intentions and Actions , 2003 .
[26] Umberto Castiello,et al. Social grasping: From mirroring to mentalizing , 2012, NeuroImage.
[27] Daniel Houser,et al. A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[28] Kai Vogeley,et al. Why we interact: On the functional role of the striatum in the subjective experience of social interaction , 2014, NeuroImage.
[29] U. Castiello,et al. Cues to intention: The role of movement information , 2011, Cognition.
[30] R. Saxe,et al. Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[31] Karl J. Friston,et al. Dissociable Neural Responses in Human Reward Systems , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[32] Alan M. Leslie,et al. Belief-desire reasoning as a process of selection , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.
[33] Patrice D. Tremoulet,et al. Perceptual causality and animacy , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[34] Beate Sodian,et al. Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning , 2007, NeuroImage.
[35] Rebecca Saxe,et al. Live face-to-face interaction during fMRI: A new tool for social cognitive neuroscience , 2010, NeuroImage.
[36] Cristina Becchio,et al. Communicative Interactions Improve Visual Detection of Biological Motion , 2011, PloS one.
[37] C. Frith,et al. Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.
[38] E. Funnell. Evidence for scripts in semantic dementia: Implications for theories of semantic memory , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[39] D. Lakens. Movement synchrony and perceived entitativity , 2010 .
[40] Lindsey J. Powell,et al. It's the Thought That Counts , 2006, Psychological science.
[41] Stefanie I. Becker,et al. Implicit false-belief processing in the human brain , 2014, NeuroImage.
[42] Béatrice de Gelder,et al. Tease or threat? Judging social interactions from bodily expressions , 2010, NeuroImage.
[43] B. Abler,et al. Motivating forces of human actions Neuroimaging reward and social interaction , 2005, Brain Research Bulletin.
[44] James K Rilling,et al. The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions , 2004, NeuroImage.
[45] Ingrid R. Olson,et al. Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes , 2010, NeuroImage.
[46] J. Searle. Consciousness and Language: Collective Intentions and Actions , 2002 .
[47] Stephen Lawrie,et al. Functional Specialization within Rostral Prefrontal Cortex (Area 10): A Meta-analysis , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[48] Riitta Hari,et al. Engagement of amygdala in third‐person view of face‐to‐face interaction , 2012, Human brain mapping.
[49] C. Frith. The social brain? , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[50] N. Akhtar,et al. Imitative learning from a third-party interaction: relations with self-recognition and perspective taking. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[51] Ivan Toni,et al. The role of immediate and final goals in action planning: An fMRI study , 2007, NeuroImage.
[52] Eleanor A. Maguire,et al. Spontaneous mentalizing during an interactive real world task: An fMRI study , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[53] Dylan D Wagner,et al. Individual Differences in the Spontaneous Recruitment of Brain Regions Supporting Mental State Understanding When Viewing Natural Social Scenes , 2011 .
[54] G. Pagnoni,et al. A Neural Basis for Social Cooperation , 2002, Neuron.
[55] N. Wright. Let's work together , 1968 .
[56] H. Walter,et al. The intentional network: How the brain reads varieties of intentions , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[57] Jason P. Mitchell,et al. Mentalizing under Uncertainty: Dissociated Neural Responses to Ambiguous and Unambiguous Mental State Inferences , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[58] A. Endress,et al. The Social Sense: Susceptibility to Others’ Beliefs in Human Infants and Adults , 2010, Science.
[59] M. Delgado,et al. Motivation-dependent responses in the human caudate nucleus. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.
[60] M. Jeannerod,et al. The motor theory of social cognition: a critique , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[61] David Baddeley,et al. 4D Super-Resolution Microscopy with Conventional Fluorophores and Single Wavelength Excitation in Optically Thick Cells and Tissues , 2011, PloS one.
[62] Annette M. E. Henderson,et al. “Let’s work together”: What do infants understand about collaborative goals? , 2011, Cognition.
[63] D. Lakens,et al. If They Move in Sync, They Must Feel in Sync: Movement Synchrony Leads to Attributions of Rapport and Entitativity , 2011 .
[64] Georgene L. Troseth,et al. Third-party social interaction and word learning from video. , 2011, Child development.
[65] Margaret Gilbert,et al. Shared intention and personal intentions , 2009 .
[66] Jean-Luc Anton,et al. Recruitment of Both the Mirror and the Mentalizing Networks When Observing Social Interactions Depicted by Point-Lights: A Neuroimaging Study , 2011, PloS one.
[67] Istvan Molnar-Szakacs,et al. Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline , 2004, NeuroImage.
[68] M. Tomasello. A Natural History of Human Thinking , 2014 .
[69] Nadim Joni Shah,et al. Minds Made for Sharing: Initiating Joint Attention Recruits Reward-related Neurocircuitry , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[70] Luca Turella,et al. Observing social interactions: The effect of gaze , 2008, Social neuroscience.
[71] N. Sebanz,et al. Do people automatically track others’ beliefs? Evidence from a continuous measure , 2014, Cognition.
[72] S. Garrod,et al. Opus: University of Bath Online Publication Store Look at Those Two! the Precuneus Role in Unattended Third-person Perspective of Social Interactions , 2022 .
[73] Karl J. Friston,et al. Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .
[74] David I. Perrett,et al. An fMRI study of joint attention experience , 2005, NeuroImage.
[75] C. Frith,et al. Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[76] C. Moore,et al. Intentional relations and social understanding , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[77] Günther Knoblich,et al. Observing shared attention modulates gaze following , 2011, Cognition.
[78] L. Young,et al. The Group-Member Mind Trade-Off , 2012, Psychological science.
[79] A. Owen,et al. Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[80] H. Bekkering,et al. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.