Effects of mother-infant social interactions on infants' subsequent contingency task performance.

20 3-month-old infants participated in a nonsocial contingency task immediately following a social interaction with their mothers. A measure of the time the dyads spent in a state of vocal turn-taking predicted individual differences in the infants' subsequent performance on the contingency task. These results parallel the social transfer effects we reported earlier in which the turn-taking dimension of social structure was experimentally manipulated to assess its effect on a subsequent nonsocial contingency task.

[1]  The mother-infant communication system , 1979 .

[2]  T. Brazelton,et al.  Early mother-infant reciprocity. , 2008, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[3]  K. Bloom,et al.  Turn taking affects the quality of infant vocalizations , 1987, Journal of Child Language.

[4]  L. Leavitt,et al.  Early cognitive development and its relation to maternal physiologic and behavioral responsiveness. , 1978, Child development.

[5]  P. Wolff The causes, controls, and organization of behavior in the neonate. , 1966, Psychological issues.

[6]  M. Lewis,et al.  Mother-infant interaction and cognitive development in twelve-week-old infants* , 1980 .

[7]  T. Brazelton,et al.  Monadic phases: A structural descriptive analysis of infant–mother face to face interaction. , 1980 .

[8]  T. Brazelton,et al.  The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. , 1978, Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry.

[9]  Stanley Feldstein,et al.  Adult-like temporal characteristics of mother-infant vocal interactions. , 1986 .

[10]  C. Trevarthen Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity , 1979 .

[11]  J. Bates,et al.  Mother–infant interaction and the development of individual differences in children's cognitive competence. , 1984 .

[12]  W. S. Millar,et al.  A study of operant conditioning under delayed reinforcement in early infancy. , 1972, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[13]  A. Fogel Cyclicity and Stability in Mother-Infant Face-to-Face Interaction: A Comment on Cohn and Tronick (1988) , 1988 .

[14]  R Bakeman,et al.  Early interaction: consequences for social and mental development at three years. , 1980, Child development.

[15]  L. Beckwith,et al.  7 – Home Environment and Cognitive Competence in Preterm Children during the First 5 Years , 1984 .

[16]  P. Dunham,et al.  Social contingency effects on subsequent perceptual-cognitive tasks in young infants. , 1989, Child development.

[17]  Roger Bakeman,et al.  Observing Interaction: An Introduction to Sequential Analysis , 1986 .

[18]  K. Kaye,et al.  The Mental and Social Life of Babies , 1982 .

[19]  J. Cohn,et al.  Mother infant face to face interaction influence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles in either partner's behavior , 1988 .

[20]  D. Muir,et al.  The effect of manipulating maternal behavior during an interaction on three- and six-month-olds' affect and attention. , 1988, Child development.

[21]  Daniel N. Stern,et al.  The First Relationship: Infant and Mother , 1977 .

[22]  N. Fox,et al.  Social perception in infants , 1985 .

[23]  G. Morgan,et al.  Infants' persistence at tasks: Relationships to cognitive functioning and early experience , 1982 .

[24]  M. Bornstein How infant and mother jointly contribute to developing cognitive competence in the child. , 1985, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  T. Field Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers , 1984 .

[26]  A. Sameroff,et al.  Familial risk and child competence. , 1983 .

[27]  E. Tronick Emotions and emotional communication in infants. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[28]  D. Stern,et al.  VOCALIZING IN UNISON AND IN ALTERNATION: TWO MODES OF COMMUNICATION WITHIN THE MOTHER‐INFANT DYAD * , 1975, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.