The perception of moving stimuli: A model of spatiotemporal coding in human vision

An empirically plausible extension of the Marr and Ullman (1981) [Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 211, 151-180 (1981)] model for coding movement direction uses the magnitude of spatial and temporal response derivatives to encode the speed as well as the direction of movement. The performance of the model is illustrated for drifting edges of different spatial scales and the properties of the components which separately compute the two derivatives are respectively compared to individual channels of the conventional pattern and flicker systems.

[1]  R. Watt,et al.  The recognition and representation of edge blur: Evidence for spatial primitives in human vision , 1983, Vision Research.

[2]  J. Movshon,et al.  Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of striate cortical neurones , 1975, Nature.

[3]  M. G. Harris,et al.  Velocity specificity of the flicker to pattern sensitivity ratio in human vision , 1980, Vision Research.

[4]  J. Kulikowski,et al.  Spatial arrangement of line, edge and grating detectors revealed by subthreshold summation. , 1973, Vision research.

[5]  R. Shapley,et al.  The receptive field organization of X-cells in the cat: Spatiotemporal coupling and asymmetry , 1984, Vision Research.

[6]  C. Enroth-Cugell,et al.  The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  A. Johnston,et al.  Invariant tuning of motion aftereffect , 1985, Vision Research.

[8]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Size Adaptation: A New Aftereffect , 1969, Science.

[9]  E. Hildreth The computation of the velocity field , 1984, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[10]  H. Wilson Spatiotemporal characterization of a transient mechanism in the human visual system , 1980, Vision Research.

[11]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Shifts in perceived size due to masking , 1983, Vision Research.

[12]  S. Ullman,et al.  The interpretation of visual motion , 1977 .

[13]  P. Thompson Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast , 1982, Vision Research.

[14]  Ellen C. Hildreth,et al.  Computations Underlying the Measurement of Visual Motion , 1984, Artif. Intell..

[15]  R. J. Watt,et al.  Mechanisms responsible for the assessment of visual location: Theory and evidence , 1983, Vision Research.

[16]  J. Movshon,et al.  Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat's striate cortex. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.

[17]  F. Campbell,et al.  The tilt after-effect: a fresh look. , 1971, Vision research.

[18]  J. Robson,et al.  Discrimination at threshold: Labelled detectors in human vision , 1981, Vision Research.

[19]  C. Enroth-Cugell,et al.  Spatio‐temporal interactions in cat retinal ganglion cells showing linear spatial summation. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  J. Bergen,et al.  A four mechanism model for threshold spatial vision , 1979, Vision Research.

[21]  O J Braddick,et al.  Low-level and high-level processes in apparent motion. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[22]  R. Bracewell The Fourier Transform and Its Applications , 1966 .

[23]  L Maffei,et al.  Spatial‐frequency characteristics of neurones of area 18 in the cat: dependence on the velocity of the visual stimulus. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[24]  D Marr,et al.  Directional selectivity and its use in early visual processing , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[25]  H. Barlow,et al.  The mechanism of directionally selective units in rabbit's retina. , 1965, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  D Marr,et al.  Theory of edge detection , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.