Spatially directed movement and neuronal activity in freely moving monkey.

The abilities to plan a series of movements and to navigate within the environment require the functions of the frontal and ventromedial temporal lobes, respectively. Neuropsychological studies posit the existence of egocentric (prefrontal) and allocentric (ventromedial temporal) spatial frames of reference that mediate these functions. To examine neural mechanisms underlying egocentric and allocentric guidance of movement, we have developed behavioral and neurophysiological techniques for freely moving monkey. In this chapter, we provide evidence that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is important for egocentric spatial tasks in both the visual and tactile modalities, but it does not contribute to performance of an allocentric spatial task. Moreover, neurophysiological recordings indicate that prefrontal neurons are involved in monitoring the spatial nature of behavioral sequences in an egocentric memory task. In contrast, hippocampal neurons are active during spatially directed locomotion, apparently reflecting the monkey's location in a testing room. This discharge is independent of the task's contingencies.

[1]  Yuan-Ye Ma,et al.  Dissociation of egocentric and allocentric spatial processing in prefrontal cortex , 2003, Neuroreport.

[2]  F. Richer,et al.  Inter-response interference contributes to the sequencing deficit in frontal lobe lesions. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  D. Stuss,et al.  The Frontal Lobes , 1986 .

[4]  Ellen Perecman,et al.  The frontal lobes revisited. , 1987 .

[5]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[6]  D. Pandya,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[7]  M. L. Rusconi,et al.  Unilateral neglect in route description , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  J. O’Keefe Do hippocampal pyramidal cells signal non‐spatial as well as spatial information? , 1999, Hippocampus.

[9]  C. Kennard,et al.  Saccadic eye movement and working memory deficits following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Prefrontal neuronal activity in rhesus monkeys performing a delayed anti-saccade task , 1993, Nature.

[11]  N Butters,et al.  Retention of Delayed-Alternation: Effect of Selective Lesions of Sulcus Principalis , 1969, Science.

[12]  M Petrides,et al.  Architecture and connections of retrosplenial area 30 in the rhesus monkey (macaca mulatta). , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[13]  K. Doya,et al.  Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  Andrew Kertesz,et al.  Localization and neuroimaging in neuropsychology , 1994 .

[15]  R U Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. , 1993, Science.

[17]  M Petrides,et al.  Impairments on nonspatial self-ordered and externally ordered working memory tasks after lesions of the mid-dorsal part of the lateral frontal cortex in the monkey , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  E. Rolls,et al.  Spatial view cells in the primate hippocampus: allocentric view not head direction or eye position or place. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  H. Niki,et al.  Prefrontal cortical unit activity and delayed alternation performance in monkeys. , 1971, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Recalling Routes around London: Activation of the Right Hippocampus in Taxi Drivers , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  W. Pohl,et al.  Dissociation of spatial discrimination deficits following frontal and parietal lesions in monkeys. , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[22]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Neuron Activity Related to Short-Term Memory , 1971, Science.

[24]  S. Funahashi,et al.  Delay-period activity in the primate prefrontal cortex encoding multiple spatial positions and their order of presentation , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[25]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[26]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Localization of function within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the rhesus monkey. , 1970, Experimental neurology.

[27]  Gianfranco Denes,et al.  A category-specific deficit of spatial representation: the case of autotopagnosia , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  E. Maguire,et al.  Topographical disorientation following unilateral temporal lobe lesions in humans , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Hippocampus and medial temporal cortex: Neuronal activity related to behavioural responses during the performance of memory tasks by primates , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[30]  R. Spinks The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe, 3rd ed. , 2000 .

[31]  Jon Driver,et al.  Spatial working memory deficit in unilateral neglect , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  D. S. Olton,et al.  Effects of fornix transection and cingulate cortical ablation on spatial memory in rhesus monkeys , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  P S Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Face-selective neurons during passive viewing and working memory performance of rhesus monkeys: evidence for intrinsic specialization of neuronal coding. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  B. Milner,et al.  Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  M. Stark,et al.  Impairment of an Egocentric Map of Locations: Implications for Perception and Action , 1996 .