Adaptational and learning processes during human split-belt locomotion: interaction between central mechanisms and afferent input

Split-belt locomotion (i.e., walking with unequal leg speeds) requires a rapid adaptation of biome-chanical parameters and therefore of leg muscle electromyographic (EMG) activity. This adaptational process during the first strides of asymmetric gait as well as learning effects induced by repetition were studied in 11 healthy volunteers. Subjects were switched from slow (0.5 m/s) symmetric gait to split-belt locomotion with speeds of 0.5 m/s and 1.5 m/s, respectively. All subjects were observed to adapt in a similar way: (1) during the first trial, adaptation required about 12–15 strides. This was achieved by an increase in stride cycle duration, i.e., an increase in swing duration on the fast side and an increase in support duration on the slow side. (2) Adaptation of leg extensor and flexor EMG activity paralleled the changes of biomechanical parameters. During the first strides, muscle activity was enhanced with no increase in coactivity of antagonistic leg muscles. (3) A motor learning effect was seen when the same paradigm was repeated a few minutes later — interrupted by symmetric locomotion — as adaptation to the split-belt speeds was achieved within 1–3 strides. (4) This short-time learning effect did not occur in the “mirror” condition when the slow and fast sides were inverted. In this case adaptation again required 12–15 strides. A close link between central and proprioceptive mechanisms of interlimb coordination is suggested to underlie the adaptational processes during split-belt conditions. It can be assumed that, as in quadrupedal locomotion of the cat, human bipedal locomotion involves separate locomotor generators to provide the flexibility demanded. The present results suggest that side-specific proprioceptive information regarding the dynamics of the movement is necessary to adjust the centrally generated locomotor activity for both legs to the actual needs for controlled locomotion. Although the required pattern is quickly learned, this learning effect cannot be transferred to the contralateral side.

[1]  M. Schmidt,et al.  Acylation of virol. spike glycoproteins: A feature of enveloped RNA viruses , 1982, Virology.

[2]  A Charlett,et al.  Hypothesis: age-associated changes in gait represent, in part, a tendency towards parkinsonism. , 1992, Age and ageing.

[3]  M. Gorassini,et al.  Corrective responses to loss of ground support during walking. I. Intact cats. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  B Conrad,et al.  Pathophysiological aspects of human locomotion. , 1983, Advances in neurology.

[5]  T. Brown On the nature of the fundamental activity of the nervous centres; together with an analysis of the conditioning of rhythmic activity in progression, and a theory of the evolution of function in the nervous system , 1914, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  D. Marr A theory of cerebellar cortex , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  P. Jacobs,et al.  Involuntary stepping after chronic spinal cord injury. Evidence for a central rhythm generator for locomotion in man. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  D. Winter,et al.  Biomechanical walking pattern changes in the fit and healthy elderly. , 1990, Physical therapy.

[9]  V. Dietz,et al.  Normal and impaired development of children's gait. , 1984, Human neurobiology.

[10]  K. Pearson,et al.  Corrective responses to loss of ground support during walking. II. Comparison of intact and chronic spinal cats. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  M. Hallett,et al.  Motor learning in patients with cerebellar dysfunction. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  S. Rossignol,et al.  The locomotion of the low spinal cat. II. Interlimb coordination. , 1980, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[13]  V. Dietz,et al.  Balance control in Parkinson's disease , 1993 .

[14]  V. Dietz,et al.  Locomotor activity in spinal man , 1994, The Lancet.

[15]  Wiebren Zijlstra,et al.  Voluntary and involuntary adaptation of walking to temporal and spatial constraints , 1995 .

[16]  S. Grillner Control of Locomotion in Bipeds, Tetrapods, and Fish , 1981 .

[17]  J Quintern,et al.  Electrophysiological studies of gait in spasticity and rigidity. Evidence that altered mechanical properties of muscle contribute to hypertonia. , 1981, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  B. Bussel,et al.  Myoclonus in a patient with spinal cord transection. Possible involvement of the spinal stepping generator. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  C. R. Gordon,et al.  Adaptive plasticity in the control of locomotor trajectory , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  W T Thach,et al.  The cerebellum and the adaptive coordination of movement. , 1992, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  S. Grillner,et al.  Peripheral feedback mechanisms acting on the central pattern generators for locomotion in fish and cat. , 1981, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[22]  J. Duysens,et al.  Human neuronal interlimb coordination during split-belt locomotion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  A. Thorstensson,et al.  Adaptability in frequency and amplitude of leg movements during human locomotion at different speeds. , 1987, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[24]  R. Norlin,et al.  Development of Gait in Spastic Children with Cerebral Palsy , 1986, Journal of pediatric orthopedics.

[25]  R. Olshen,et al.  The development of mature gait. , 1980, The Journal of bone and joint surgery. American volume.

[26]  T. Brown The intrinsic factors in the act of progression in the mammal , 1911 .

[27]  V. Dietz,et al.  Locomotor capacity of spinal cord in paraplegic patients , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[28]  S. Rossignol,et al.  Enhancement of locomotor recovery following spinal cord injury. , 1994, Current opinion in neurology.

[29]  M. Udo,et al.  A new learning paradigm: adaptive changes in interlimb coordination during perturbed locomotion in decerebrate cats , 1993, Neuroscience Research.