Substance and artifact in the higher-order factors of the Big Five.

J. M. Digman (1997) proposed that the Big Five personality traits showed a higher-order structure with 2 factors he labeled alpha and beta. These factors have been alternatively interpreted as heritable components of personality or as artifacts of evaluative bias. Using structural equation modeling, the authors reanalyzed data from a cross-national twin study and from American cross-observer studies and analyzed new multimethod data from a German twin study. In all analyses, artifact models outperformed substance models by root-mean-square error of approximation criteria, but models combining both artifact and substance were slightly better. These findings suggest that the search for the biological basis of personality traits may be more profitably focused on the 5 factors themselves and their specific facets, especially in monomethod studies.

[1]  S G West,et al.  Consensus, self-other agreement, and accuracy in personality judgment: an introduction. , 1993, Journal of personality.

[2]  P. Bentler Comparative Fit Indices in Structural Models , 1990 .

[3]  R. McCrae,et al.  An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. , 1992, Journal of personality.

[4]  Robert R. McCrae,et al.  The NEO–PI–3: A More Readable Revised NEO Personality Inventory , 2005, Journal of personality assessment.

[5]  The Five-Factor Personality Inventory: Assessing the Big Five by means of brief and concrete statements. , 2002 .

[6]  R. McCrae,et al.  Sources of structure: genetic, environmental, and artifactual influences on the covariation of personality traits. , 2001, Journal of personality.

[7]  Delroy L. Paulhus,et al.  Egoistic and Moralistic Biases in Self-Perception: The Interplay of Self-Deceptive Styles With Basic Traits and Motives , 1998 .

[8]  L. A. Pervin Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research , 1992 .

[9]  Meike Bartels,et al.  Twins and the study of rater (dis)agreement. , 2007, Psychological methods.

[10]  Marco Perugini,et al.  Big Five Assessment , 2002 .

[11]  Kristian E Markon,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Delineating the Structure of Normal and Abnormal Personality: An Integrative Hierarchical Approach , 2004 .

[12]  S. West,et al.  Towards understanding assessments of the big five: multitrait-multimethod analyses of convergent and discriminant validity across measurement occasion and type of observer. , 2004, Journal of personality.

[13]  R. Plomin,et al.  Why are children in the same family so different from one another? , 1987, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[14]  J. Forgas,et al.  Recent advances in social psychology : an international perspective , 1989 .

[15]  R. Krueger The structure of common mental disorders. , 1999, Archives of general psychiatry.

[16]  Paul T. Costa,et al.  Age Trends and Age Norms for the NEO Personality Inventory-3 in Adolescents and Adults , 2005, Assessment.

[17]  K. Jang,et al.  The behavioral genetics of personality disorder. , 2008, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[18]  C. Rogers On Becoming a Person: A Therapist's View of Psychotherapy , 1961 .

[19]  K. Jang The Behavioral Genetics of Psychopathology: A Clinical Guide , 2005 .

[20]  C. DeYoung Higher-order factors of the Big Five in a multi-informant sample. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  P. Costa,et al.  Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO-Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) , 1992 .

[22]  Niels G. Waller,et al.  From adorable to worthless: implicit and self‐report structure of highly evaluative personality descriptors , 2002 .

[23]  J. Desmond,et al.  An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[24]  Hans J. Eysenck,et al.  Four ways five factors are not basic , 1992 .

[25]  吉村公雄 Reliability and validity of a Japanese version of the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) : A population-based survey in Aomori prefecture , 1998 .

[26]  G Saucier,et al.  Lexical studies of indigenous personality factors: premises, products, and prospects. , 2001, Journal of personality.

[27]  Robert R. McCrae,et al.  Heritability of Facet-Level Traits in a Cross-Cultural Twin Sample: Support for a Hierarchical Model of Personality , 1998 .

[28]  J. Loehlin,et al.  Genes, Evolution, and Personality , 2001, Behavior genetics.

[29]  J. M. Digman Higher-order factors of the Big Five. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  P. Costa,et al.  The five-factor theory of personality. , 2008 .

[31]  R. McCrae,et al.  Heritability of facet-level traits in a cross-cultural twin sample: support for a hierarchical model of personality. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  D. McAdams,et al.  A new Big Five: fundamental principles for an integrative science of personality. , 2006, The American psychologist.

[33]  M. Browne,et al.  Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit , 1992 .

[34]  F. Ostendorf,et al.  NEO-Persönlichkeitsinventar nach Costa und McCrae: NEO-PI-R ; Manual , 2004 .

[35]  P. Borkenau,et al.  Personality Processes and Individual Differences I Genetic and Environmental Influences on Observed Personality: Evidence from the German Observational Study of Adult Twins , 2022 .

[36]  Paul T. Costa,et al.  Discriminant Validity of NEO-PIR Facet Scales , 1992 .

[37]  A. Maslow Motivation and Personality , 1954 .

[38]  Frank M. Spinath,et al.  Behavioral genetics of the higher-order factors of the Big Five , 2006 .

[39]  G. Cumming,et al.  Inference by eye: confidence intervals and how to read pictures of data. , 2005, The American psychologist.

[40]  R. Riemann,et al.  The Jena Twin Registry and the Jena Twin Study of Social Attitudes (JeTSSA). , 2006, Twin research and human genetics : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies.

[41]  J. H. Steiger,et al.  The insidious effects of failing to include design-driven correlated residuals in latent-variable covariance structure analysis. , 2007, Psychological methods.

[42]  Jordan B. Peterson,et al.  Higher-order factors of the Big Five predict conformity: Are there neuroses of health? , 2002 .

[43]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  Trait as default: extraversion, subjective well-being, and the distinction between neutral and positive events. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[44]  Timothy J Trull,et al.  An evaluation of evaluative personality terms: a comparison of the big seven and five-factor model in predicting psychopathology. , 2005, Psychological assessment.

[45]  David C. Funder,et al.  Lay Conceptions of the Five-Factor Model and its Indicators , 1998 .

[46]  Richard E. Lucas,et al.  Extraversion and emotional reactivity. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[47]  On the invalidity of validity scales: evidence from self-reports and observer ratings in volunteer samples. , 2000 .

[48]  Peter Becker,et al.  Beyond the Big Five , 1999 .

[49]  P. Borkenau,et al.  German Observational Study of Adult Twins (GOSAT): a multimodal investigation of personality, temperament and cognitive ability. , 2002, Twin research : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies.

[50]  Y. Ono,et al.  Is the genetic structure of human personality universal? A cross-cultural twin study from North America, Europe, and Asia. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  R. McCrae,et al.  On the invalidity of validity scales: evidence from self-reports and observer ratings in volunteer samples. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[52]  P. Costa,et al.  Positive and Negative Valence within the Five-Factor Model , 1995 .